Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 6 |
Error Patterns | 6 |
Age Differences | 3 |
Child Development | 2 |
Cognitive Development | 2 |
Prediction | 2 |
Preschool Children | 2 |
Adults | 1 |
Beliefs | 1 |
Children | 1 |
Coding | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 6 |
Author
Andrews, Benjamin J. | 1 |
Apperly, Ian A. | 1 |
Bisanz, Gay L. | 1 |
Brenneman, Kimberly | 1 |
Craig, Grace J. | 1 |
Deak, Gedeon O. | 1 |
Diamond, Adele | 1 |
George, Nathan R. | 1 |
Goksun, Tilbe | 1 |
Golinkoff, Roberta M. | 1 |
Grant, Jay | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 5 |
Reports - Research | 5 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 2 |
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Goksun, Tilbe; George, Nathan R.; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Golinkoff, Roberta M. – Child Development, 2013
How do children evaluate complex causal events? This study investigates preschoolers' representation of "force dynamics" in causal scenes, asking whether (a) children understand how single and dual forces impact an object's movement and (b) this understanding varies across cause types (Cause, Enable, Prevent). Three-and-a half- to…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Cognitive Processes, Child Development, Motion
Apperly, Ian A.; Warren, Frances; Andrews, Benjamin J.; Grant, Jay; Todd, Sophie – Child Development, 2011
On belief-desire reasoning tasks, children first pass tasks involving true belief before those involving false belief, and tasks involving positive desire before those involving negative desire. The current study examined belief-desire reasoning in participants old enough to pass all such tasks. Eighty-three 6- to 11-year-olds and 20 adult…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Developmental Continuity, Cognitive Development, Child Development

Diamond, Adele – Child Development, 1985
Twenty-five infants were tested every two weeks on the AB Object Permanence Task, from the time they first reached for a hidden object until they were 12 months old. Results indicate that the AB provides an index of the ability to carry out an intention based on stored information despite a conflicting habitual tendency. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Error Patterns, Individual Differences, Infant Behavior

Craig, Grace J.; And Others – Child Development, 1973
A level prediction task, in the context of Piaget's conservation-of-liquid problem, was used to analyze the regularities of incompetence'' in the nonconserving or noncompensating child. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Compensation (Concept), Conservation (Concept)

Deak, Gedeon O.; Ray, Shanna D.; Brenneman, Kimberly – Child Development, 2003
Two experiments examined the communicative bases of preschoolers' object appearance-reality (AR) errors. Found that AR performance correlated positively with performance on a control test with the same discourse structure but nondeceptive stimuli, and on a naming test. Overall findings indicated that the discourse structure of AR tests elicits a…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Error Patterns, Language Skills

Bisanz, Gay L.; And Others – Child Development, 1984
Focuses on differences occurring with age and reading skill in the use of phonemic codes in short-term retention tasks where stimuli were presented visually. Subjects were groups of average readers in grades two, four, and six; superior readers in grade four; and disabled readers in grades four and six from three public schools. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students