Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 2 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 5 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 10 |
Descriptor
Attribution Theory | 11 |
Beliefs | 11 |
Theory of Mind | 11 |
Young Children | 5 |
Foreign Countries | 3 |
Measures (Individuals) | 3 |
Preschool Children | 3 |
Task Analysis | 3 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Cognitive Ability | 2 |
Cognitive Development | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Anne Havard | 1 |
Bayliss, Andrew P. | 1 |
Becker, Stefanie I. | 1 |
Campos, R. | 1 |
Caroline E. Harriott | 1 |
Ceolaro, Chiara | 1 |
Christopher B. Jaeger | 1 |
Conry-Murray, Clare | 1 |
Daniel T. Levin | 1 |
Di Dio, Cinzia | 1 |
Dux, Paul E. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 11 |
Reports - Research | 11 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 2 |
Higher Education | 2 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Denmark | 1 |
France (Paris) | 1 |
Italy | 1 |
Sweden | 1 |
Texas | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Expressive One Word Picture… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Jacqueline D. Woolley; Paola A. Baca; Kelsey A. Kelley – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2024
Superstitious behaviors persist across time, culture, and age. Although often considered irrational and even potentially harmful, superstitions have recently been shown to have positive effects on stress levels, confidence, and ultimately, performance. However, it remains unclear how people conceive of superstitious behaviors, specifically,…
Descriptors: Children, College Students, Beliefs, Theory of Mind
Lewis J. Baker; Hongyue Li; Hugo Hammond; Christopher B. Jaeger; Anne Havard; Jonathan D. Lane; Caroline E. Harriott; Daniel T. Levin – Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 2024
As a wide variety of intelligent technologies become part of everyday life, researchers have explored how people conceptualize agents that in some ways act and think like living things but are clearly machines. Much of this work draws upon the idea that people readily default to generalizing human-like properties to such agents, and only pare back…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Psychological Patterns, Abstract Reasoning, Attribution Theory
Campos, R.; Martínez-Castilla, P.; Sotillo, M. – Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 2017
Background: Individuals with Williams syndrome (WS) show difficulties in attributing false beliefs, whereas they are better at attributing emotions. This study examines whether being asked about the emotion linked to a false belief, instead of explicitly about the belief, facilitates performance on theory of mind (ToM) tasks. Method: Thirty…
Descriptors: Intellectual Disability, Young Children, Attribution Theory, Beliefs
Hansen, Mikkel B.; Petersen, Esben N.; Poulsen, Arne; Salès-Wuillemin, Edith – Discourse Processes: A multidisciplinary journal, 2017
The third-person belief ascription, "Marie believes that the contract is in the cabinet," may engender two interpretations: (1) It neutrally describes what is on Marie's mind and (2) it offers indirect evidence about reality, committing the speaker to the cabinet as the most likely location. The circumstances that lead to the evidential…
Descriptors: Beliefs, Attribution Theory, Theory of Mind, Experiments
Di Dio, Cinzia; Isernia, Sara; Ceolaro, Chiara; Marchetti, Antonella; Massaro, Davide – SAGE Open, 2018
The study of social cognition involves the attribution of states of mind to humans, as well as, quite recently, to nonhuman creatures, like God. Some studies support the role of social cognition in religious beliefs, whereas others ascribe religious beliefs to an ontological knowledge bias. The present study compares these distinct approaches in…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Social Cognition, Religion, Beliefs
Miller, Scott A. – Infant and Child Development, 2013
This research examined two questions: effects of deception on children's understanding of second-order false belief, and possible effects of number of siblings on second-order performance. Kindergarten children responded to 3 second-order problems that varied in the presence and the nature of deception. Performance was better on the problems…
Descriptors: Deception, Beliefs, Theory of Mind, Kindergarten
Tompkins, Virginia; Farrar, M. Jeffrey; Guo, Ying – Journal of Genetic Psychology, 2013
The authors examined the relationship between number of siblings and false belief understanding (FBU) in 94 low-income 4-5-year-olds. Previous research with middle-income children has shown a positive association between number of siblings and FBU. However, it is unclear whether having multiple siblings in low-income families is related to better…
Descriptors: Vocabulary Skills, Siblings, Preschool Children, Low Income
Meristo, Marek; Morgan, Gary; Geraci, Alessandra; Iozzi, Laura; Hjelmquist, Erland; Surian, Luca; Siegal, Michael – Developmental Science, 2012
Based on anticipatory looking and reactions to violations of expected events, infants have been credited with "theory of mind" (ToM) knowledge that a person's search behaviour for an object will be guided by true or false beliefs about the object's location. However, little is known about the preconditions for looking patterns consistent…
Descriptors: Infants, Deafness, Hearing (Physiology), Beliefs
Conry-Murray, Clare – Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2013
Children of ages 3-5 ("N" = 62) were assessed by using standard theory-of-mind tasks and unusual belief tasks related to false information and beliefs endorsing violations of moral (welfare and fairness) and social conventional (school rules) domains. Younger children (under 5 years) did not accurately attribute unusual factual beliefs…
Descriptors: Young Children, Beliefs, Theory of Mind, Moral Values
Schneider, Dana; Bayliss, Andrew P.; Becker, Stefanie I.; Dux, Paul E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2012
The ability to attribute mental states to others is crucial for social competency. To assess mentalizing abilities, in false-belief tasks participants attempt to identify an actor's belief about an object's location as opposed to the object's actual location. Passing this test on explicit measures is typically achieved by 4 years of age, but…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Eye Movements, Task Analysis, Age Differences
de Rosnay, Marc; Pons, Francisco; Harris, Paul L.; Morrell, Julian M. B. – British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2004
This study examines the contribution of children's linguistic ability and mothers' use of mental-state language to young children's understanding of false belief and their subsequent ability to make belief-based emotion attributions. In Experiment 1, children (N = 51) were given three belief-based emotion-attribution tasks. A standard task in…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Video Technology, Mothers, Semantics