Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 16 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 45 |
Cognitive Processes | 45 |
Theories | 45 |
Children | 19 |
Cognitive Development | 19 |
Models | 14 |
Memory | 12 |
Individual Development | 10 |
Child Development | 9 |
Adults | 7 |
Concept Formation | 6 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Bigler, Rebecca S. | 2 |
Liben, Lynn S. | 2 |
Madole, Kelly L. | 2 |
Oakes, Lisa M. | 2 |
Wellman, Henry M. | 2 |
Acosta, Manuel | 1 |
Aguilar, Manuel | 1 |
Anastasi, Anne | 1 |
Anderson, Mike | 1 |
Autio, Ossi | 1 |
Bajo, M. Teresa | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 40 |
Reports - Research | 25 |
Opinion Papers | 9 |
Information Analyses | 6 |
Reports - Evaluative | 4 |
Reports - Descriptive | 3 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Adult Education | 3 |
Higher Education | 3 |
Elementary Education | 2 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Colliver, Yeshe; Fleer, Marilyn – Early Child Development and Care, 2016
Around the world, if and how young children learn through their play in early childhood education and care contexts has been the subject of much debate. Yet rarely has the debate heard from the young children themselves, often due to the pervasive belief that they do not understand learning. To redress this, a qualitative case study was conducted…
Descriptors: Play, Learning Strategies, Case Studies, Attitude Measures
Tamez, Elaine; Myerson, Joel; Hale, Sandra – Intelligence, 2012
According to the cognitive cascade hypothesis, age-related slowing results in decreased working memory, which in turn affects higher-order cognition. Because recent studies show complex associative learning correlates highly with fluid intelligence, the present study examined the role of complex associative learning in cognitive cascade models of…
Descriptors: Intelligence, Associative Learning, Short Term Memory, Cognitive Processes
Fox, Allison M.; Reid, Corinne L.; Anderson, Mike; Richardson, Cassandra; Bishop, Dorothy V. M. – Developmental Science, 2012
According to the rapid auditory processing theory, the ability to parse incoming auditory information underpins learning of oral and written language. There is wide variation in this low-level perceptual ability, which appears to follow a protracted developmental course. We studied the development of rapid auditory processing using event-related…
Descriptors: Intervals, Written Language, Oral Language, Correlation
Mandler, Jean M. – Cognitive Science, 2012
A theory of how concept formation begins is presented that accounts for conceptual activity in the first year of life, shows how increasing conceptual complexity comes about, and predicts the order in which new types of information accrue to the conceptual system. In a compromise between nativist and empiricist views, it offers a single…
Descriptors: Concept Formation, Theories, Cognitive Processes, Attention
Stanovich, Keith E.; West, Richard F.; Toplak, Maggie E. – Developmental Review, 2011
Drawing developmental predictions from dual-process theories is more complex than is commonly realized. Overly simplified predictions drawn from such models may lead to premature rejection of the dual process approach as one of many tools for understanding cognitive development. Misleading predictions can be avoided by paying attention to several…
Descriptors: Prediction, Cognitive Development, Theories, Task Analysis
Ortega, Almudena; Gomez-Ariza, Carlos J.; Roman, Patricia; Bajo, M. Teresa – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
Although memory inhibition seems to underlie retrieval-induced forgetting (RIF), there is some controversy about the precise nature of this effect. Because normal RIF is observed in people with deficits in executive control (i.e., older adults), some have proposed that an automatic-like inhibitory process is responsible for the effect. On the…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Young Adults, Older Adults, Memory
Cordova, Alberto; Gabbard, Carl – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2012
Theory suggests that the vision-for-perception and vision-for-action processing streams operate under very different temporal constraints (Glover, 2004; Goodale, Jackobson, & Keillor, 1994; Graham, Bradshaw, & Davis, 1998; Hu, Eagleson, & Goodale, 1999). With the present study, children and young adults were asked to estimate how far a cued target…
Descriptors: Cues, Vision, Theories, Statistical Analysis
Cimpian, Andrei; Gelman, Susan A.; Brandone, Amanda C. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2010
Under what circumstances do people agree that a kind-referring generic sentence (e.g., "Swans are beautiful") is true? We hypothesised that theory-based considerations are sufficient, independently of prevalence/frequency information, to lead to acceptance of a generic statement. To provide evidence for this general point, we focused on…
Descriptors: Hypothesis Testing, Sentences, Thinking Skills, Theories
Feeney, Aidan; Coley, John D.; Crisp, Aimee – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2010
Relevance theory (Sperber & Wilson, 1995) suggests that people expend cognitive effort when processing information in proportion to the cognitive effects to be gained from doing so. This theory has been used to explain how people apply their knowledge appropriately when evaluating category-based inductive arguments (Medin, Coley, Storms, &…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Logical Thinking, Experiments, Foreign Countries
Benjamin, Aaron S. – Psychological Review, 2010
It is widely assumed that older adults suffer a deficit in the psychological processes that underlie remembering of contextual or source information. This conclusion is based in large part on empirical interactions, including disordinal ones, that reveal differential effects of manipulations of memory strength on recognition in young and old…
Descriptors: Memory, Age Differences, Context Effect, Aging (Individuals)
Schutte, Anne R.; Spencer, John P. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2010
In early childhood, there is a developmental transition in spatial memory biases. Before the transition, children's memory responses are biased toward the midline of a space, while after the transition responses are biased away from midline. The Dynamic Field Theory (DFT) posits that changes in neural interaction and changes in how children…
Descriptors: Memory, Spatial Ability, Schemata (Cognition), Prediction
De Neys, Wim; Van Gelder, Elke – Developmental Science, 2009
Popular reasoning theories postulate that the ability to inhibit inappropriate beliefs lies at the heart of the human reasoning engine. Given that people's inhibitory capacities are known to rise and fall across the lifespan, we predicted that people's deductive reasoning performance would show similar curvilinear age trends. A group of children…
Descriptors: Conflict, Inhibition, Young Adults, Logical Thinking
Barry, Elaine S. – Journal of Genetic Psychology, 2007
The author investigated the importance of processing considerations within implicit memory in a developmental design. Second-graders (n = 87) and college students (n = 81) completed perceptual (word stem completion) and conceptual (category generation) implicit memory tests after studying target items either nonsemantically (read) or semantically…
Descriptors: College Students, Grade 2, Semantics, Age Differences
Ellefson, Michelle R.; Shapiron, Laura R.; Chater, Nick – Cognitive Development, 2006
Switching between tasks produces decreases in performance as compared to repeating the same task. Asymmetrical switch costs occur when switching between two tasks of unequal difficulty. This asymmetry occurs because the cost is greater when switching to the less difficult task than when switching to the more difficult task. Various theories about…
Descriptors: Children, Difficulty Level, Adults, Age Differences

van der Maas, Han L. J.; Jansen, Brenda R. J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2003
Predictions about reaction times (RT) from Siegler's model were tested for the balance scale task with 6- to 22-year-olds. Regression analyses provided additional knowledge of the rules. Rule II was reformulated as a rule that always involves the encoding but not always the correct application of the distance rule. RTs provided evidence for use of…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes