Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 8 |
Classification | 8 |
Familiarity | 8 |
Novelty (Stimulus Dimension) | 4 |
Adults | 3 |
Child Development | 3 |
Cognitive Development | 3 |
Habituation | 3 |
Infants | 3 |
Knowledge Level | 3 |
Children | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 3 |
Developmental Psychology | 2 |
Infant and Child Development | 1 |
Journal of Cognition and… | 1 |
Journal of Vocational Behavior | 1 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 8 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
High Schools | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Switzerland | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
MacArthur Communicative… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Ruba, Ashley L.; Meltzoff, Andrew N.; Repacholi, Betty M. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
Accurate perception of emotional (facial) expressions is an essential social skill. It is currently debated whether emotion categorization in infancy emerges in a "broad-to-narrow" pattern and the degree to which language influences this process. We used an habituation paradigm to explore (a) whether 14- and 18-month-old infants perceive…
Descriptors: Infants, Nonverbal Communication, Emotional Response, Toddlers
Hupp, Julie M. – Infant and Child Development, 2015
Attention allocation in word learning may vary developmentally based on the novelty of the object. It has been suggested that children differentially learn verbs based on the novelty of the agent, but adults do not because they automatically infer the object's category and thus treat it like a familiar object. The current research examined…
Descriptors: Vocabulary Development, Adults, Children, Nouns
Casasola, Marianella; Park, Youjeong – Child Development, 2013
Two experiments examined infants' ability to form a spatial category when habituated to few (only 2) or many (6) exemplars of a spatial relation. Sixty-four infants of 10 months and 64 infants of 14 months were habituated to dynamic events in which a toy was placed in a consistent spatial relation ("in" or "on") to a referent…
Descriptors: Infants, Spatial Ability, Classification, Child Development
Martarelli, Corinna S.; Mast, Fred W. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2013
Children aged 3 to 8 years old and adults were tested on a reality–fantasy distinction task. They had to judge whether particular entities were real or fantastical, and response times were collected. We further manipulated whether the entity is a specific character or a generic fantastical entity. The results indicate that children, unlike adults,…
Descriptors: Young Children, Adults, Fantasy, Realism
Liang, Belle; Spencer, Renee; Brogan, Deirdre; Corral, Macarena – Journal of Vocational Behavior, 2008
This study compared perceptions of mentoring relationships among early adolescents, middle adolescents, and emerging adults. In ten focus groups, 56 middle school, high school, and college students described relational experiences that were analyzed thematically. Differences in the characteristics of the mentors nominated by the youth across the…
Descriptors: Mentors, Interpersonal Relationship, Early Adolescents, Adolescents

Younger, Barbara A.; Fearing, Dru D. – Child Development, 1999
Three experiments used a familiarization/novelty or a habituation/dishabituation procedure to examine developmental change in infants' tendency to parse exemplars into separate categories. Results indicated that 10-month olds appeared to form differentiated categories, whereas 4- and 7-month olds formed a single category to include the range of…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Processes, Familiarity

Farrar, Michael Jeffrey; And Others – Child Development, 1992
In one experiment, second and fourth graders used more categorical information when they made inferences than did preschoolers. In two other experiments, second graders, but not preschoolers, distinguished between categorical information and appearance when they made inferences about known concepts and familiar properties. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Development, Elementary Education

Johnson, Kathy E.; Scott, Paul; Mervis, Carolyn B. – Developmental Psychology, 1997
Four studies examined developmental differences in the representation of basic-subordinate inclusion relationships in three-, five-, and seven-year olds and undergraduates. Found that even three-year olds showed rudimentary knowledge of the asymmetry of inclusion. There was a marked developmental gap between producing subordinate category names…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Child Development, Children