Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 15 |
Descriptor
Adults | 16 |
Experimental Psychology | 16 |
Spatial Ability | 16 |
Visual Stimuli | 9 |
Cognitive Processes | 6 |
Task Analysis | 6 |
Short Term Memory | 5 |
Comparative Analysis | 4 |
Correlation | 4 |
Cues | 4 |
Experiments | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 8 |
Journal of Experimental Child… | 3 |
Journal of Experimental… | 2 |
Infancy | 1 |
Journal of Experimental… | 1 |
Journal of Experimental… | 1 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 16 |
Reports - Research | 13 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 4 |
Adult Education | 3 |
Elementary Education | 2 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
United Kingdom | 2 |
California | 1 |
Massachusetts | 1 |
Portugal | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Wechsler Adult Intelligence… | 1 |
Wide Range Achievement Test | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Shaki, Samuel; Fischer, Martin H.; Gobel, Silke M. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Western adults associate small numbers with left space and large numbers with right space. Where does this pervasive spatial-numerical association come from? In this study, we first recorded directional counting preferences in adults with different reading experiences (left to right, right to left, mixed, and illiterate) and observed a clear…
Descriptors: Elementary School Students, Student Attitudes, Early Reading, Comparative Analysis
Sauter, Megan; Uttal, David H.; Alman, Amanda Schaal; Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Levine, Susan C. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
This article examines two issues: the role of gesture in the communication of spatial information and the relation between communication and mental representation. Children (8-10 years) and adults walked through a space to learn the locations of six hidden toy animals and then explained the space to another person. In Study 1, older children and…
Descriptors: Animals, Nonverbal Communication, Spatial Ability, Children
Chen, Fu-Chen; Stoffregen, Thomas A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2012
Mariners actively adjust their body orientation in response to ship motion. On a ship at sea, we evaluated relations between standing postural activity and the performance of a precision aiming task. Standing participants (experienced mariners) maintained the beam from a handheld laser on a target. Targets were large or small, thereby varying the…
Descriptors: Workstations, Cognitive Processes, Task Analysis, Priming
Giudice, Nicholas A.; Betty, Maryann R.; Loomis, Jack M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
This research examined whether visual and haptic map learning yield functionally equivalent spatial images in working memory, as evidenced by similar encoding bias and updating performance. In 3 experiments, participants learned 4-point routes either by seeing or feeling the maps. At test, blindfolded participants made spatial judgments about the…
Descriptors: Evidence, Short Term Memory, Maps, Spatial Ability
Vo, Melissa L. -H.; Wolfe, Jeremy M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
One might assume that familiarity with a scene or previous encounters with objects embedded in a scene would benefit subsequent search for those items. However, in a series of experiments we show that this is not the case: When participants were asked to subsequently search for multiple objects in the same scene, search performance remained…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Memory, Spatial Ability, Guidance
Raw, Rachael K.; Kountouriotis, Georgios K.; Mon-Williams, Mark; Wilkie, Richard M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Old age is associated with poorer movement skill, as indexed by reduced speed and accuracy. Nevertheless, reductions in speed and accuracy can also reflect compensation as well as deficit. We used a manual tracing and a driving task to identify generalized spatial and temporal compensations and deficits associated with old age. In Experiment 1,…
Descriptors: Older Adults, Psychomotor Skills, Computer Simulation, Cognitive Processes
Wood, Justin N. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Visual working memory (VWM) is widely thought to contain specialized buffers for retaining spatial and object information: a "spatial-object architecture." However, studies of adults, infants, and nonhuman animals show that visual cognition builds on core knowledge systems that retain more specialized representations: (1) spatiotemporal…
Descriptors: Evidence, Architecture, Infants, Short Term Memory
Leblanc, Emilie; Jolicoeur, Pierre – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
Previous research on the control of visuospatial attention showed that overlearned symbols like arrows have the potential to induce involuntary shifts of attention. Following work on the role of attentional control settings and of the content of working memory in the involuntary deployment of visuospatial attention, Pratt and Hommel (2003) found…
Descriptors: Proximity, Attention Control, Short Term Memory, Cues
Jones, Manon W.; Branigan, Holly P.; Parra, Mario A.; Logie, Robert H. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
The ability to learn visual-phonological associations is a unique predictor of word reading, and individuals with developmental dyslexia show impaired ability in learning these associations. In this study, we compared developmentally dyslexic and nondyslexic adults on their ability to form cross-modal associations (or "bindings") based…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Dyslexia, Predictor Variables, Associative Learning
Kolinsky, Regine; Verhaeghe, Arlette; Fernandes, Tania; Mengarda, Elias Jose; Grimm-Cabral, Loni; Morais, Jose – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2011
To examine whether enantiomorphy (i.e., the ability to discriminate lateral mirror images) is influenced by the acquisition of a written system that incorporates mirrored letters (e.g., b and d), unschooled illiterate adults were compared with people reading the Latin alphabet, namely, both schooled literate adults and unschooled adults…
Descriptors: Literacy Education, Illiteracy, Latin, Visual Discrimination
Hommuk, Karita; Bachmann, Talis – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2009
The problem of feature binding has been examined under conditions of distributed attention or with spatially dispersed stimuli. We studied binding by asking whether selective attention to a feature of a masked object enables perceptual access to the other features of that object using conditions in which spatial attention was directed at a single…
Descriptors: Attention, Identification, Visual Perception, Spatial Ability
Zelanti, Pierre S.; Droit-Volet, Sylvie – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Adults and children (5- and 8-year-olds) performed a temporal bisection task with either auditory or visual signals and either a short (0.5-1.0s) or long (4.0-8.0s) duration range. Their working memory and attentional capacities were assessed by a series of neuropsychological tests administered in both the auditory and visual modalities. Results…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Auditory Stimuli, Short Term Memory, Adults
Thaler, Lore; Todd, James T. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
Visual information can specify spatial layout with respect to the observer (egocentric) or with respect to an external frame of reference (allocentric). People can use both of these types of visual spatial information to guide their hands. The question arises if movements based on egocentric and movements based on allocentric visual information…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Testing, Visual Perception, Brain
Maus, Gerrit W.; Nijhawan, Romi – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2009
When a moving object abruptly disappears, this profoundly influences its localization by the visual system. In Experiment 1, 2 aligned objects moved across the screen, and 1 of them abruptly disappeared. Observers reported seeing the objects misaligned at the time of the offset, with the continuing object leading. Experiment 2 showed that the…
Descriptors: Adults, Visual Perception, Experiments, Experimental Psychology
Wyble, Brad; Bowman, Howard; Potter, Mary C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2009
Transient attention to a visually salient cue enhances processing of a subsequent target in the same spatial location between 50 to 150 ms after cue onset (K. Nakayama & M. Mackeben, 1989). Do stimuli from a categorically defined target set, such as letters or digits, also generate transient attention? Participants reported digit targets among…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Attention, Cognitive Processes, Cues
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2