NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 31 to 45 of 64 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kreiner, Hamutal; Sturt, Patrick; Garrod, Simon – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
Readers immediately slow down when an anaphor (e.g. herself) refers to an antecedent that mismatches in stereotypical gender (e.g. minister). The mismatch-cost has been attributed to a clash between the gender of the pronoun and the gender associated with the stereotypical role noun. However, the nature of such stereotypical gender is still…
Descriptors: Sentences, Nouns, Sex Role, Human Body
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ameel, Eef; Malt, Barbara; Storms, Gert – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
Despite arguments for the relative ease of learning common noun meanings, semantic development continues well past the early years of language acquisition even for names of concrete objects. We studied evolution of the use of common nouns during later lexical development. Children aged 5-14 years and adults named common household objects and their…
Descriptors: University Presses, Semantics, Nouns, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Snedeker, Jesse; Yuan, Sylvia – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
Prior studies of ambiguity resolution in young children have found that children rely heavily on lexical information but persistently fail to use referential constraints in online parsing [Trueswell, J.C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N.M., & Logrip, M.L, (1999). The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children.…
Descriptors: Sentences, Cues, Form Classes (Languages), Figurative Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Melinger, Alissa; Koenig, Jean-Pierre – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
This paper presents three naming experiments designed to investigate whether the activation levels of syntactic features associated with lexical items, specifically part-of-speech information, can influence lexical processes. Naming preferences for orthographically ambiguous but phonologically distinct English nouns and verbs, such as "convict"…
Descriptors: Semantics, Nouns, Vocabulary Development, Form Classes (Languages)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Thothathiri, Malathi; Snedeker, Jesse – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
We report two sets of experiments that demonstrate syntactic priming from comprehension to comprehension in young children. Children acted out double-object and prepositional-object dative sentences while we monitored their eye movements. We measured whether hearing one type of dative as a prime influenced children's online interpretation of…
Descriptors: Comprehension, Syntax, Sentences, Verbs
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Staub, Adrian – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Two eye movement experiments examined effects on syntactic reanalysis when the correct analysis was briefly entertained at an earlier point in the sentence. In Experiment 1, participants read sentences containing a noun phrase coordination/clausal coordination ambiguity, while in Experiment 2 they read sentences containing a subordinate clause…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Sentences, Nouns, Eye Movements
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dabrowska, Ewa – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
An experiment testing adult Polish speakers' ability to supply dative forms of unfamiliar nouns revealed strong effects of type frequency (performance was better on inflections that apply to large classes) and neighbourhood density (participants were more likely to supply the target inflection with nonce nouns belonging to densely populated…
Descriptors: Nouns, Vocabulary Development, Adults, Polish
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Raffray, Claudine N.; Pickering, Martin J.; Branigan, Holly P. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Noun-noun combinations like "dog scarf" are common in everyday discourse but often have more than one interpretation. How do language users arrive at an interpretation of the relationship between the two nouns? This paper reports three expression-picture matching experiments that used a priming paradigm to investigate the influence of modifier and…
Descriptors: Nouns, Language Usage, Semantics, Pictorial Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Schweppe, Judith; Rummer, Ralf – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
The general idea of language-based accounts of short-term memory is that retention of linguistic materials is based on representations within the language processing system. In the present sentence recall study, we address the question whether the assumption of shared representations holds for morphosyntactic information (here: grammatical gender…
Descriptors: Grammar, Short Term Memory, Sentences, Nouns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Frisson, S.; Frazier, L. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2005
Two eye-tracking experiments investigated the processing of mass nouns used as count nouns and count nouns used as mass nouns. Following Copestake and Briscoe (1995), the basic or underived sense of a word was treated as the input to a derivational rule (''grinding'' or ''portioning'') which produced the derived sense as output. It was…
Descriptors: Semantics, Nouns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Damian, Markus F.; Dumay, Nicolas – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Current accounts of spoken production debate the extent to which speakers plan ahead. Here, we investigated whether the scope of phonological planning is influenced by changes in time pressure constraints. The first experiment used a picture-word interference task and showed that picture naming latencies were shorter when word distractors shared…
Descriptors: Nouns, English (Second Language), Models, Phonology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Badecker, William; Kuminiak, Frantisek – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
The experimental studies presented in this paper exploit agreement attraction in order to examine the mechanisms underlying the production of subject-verb agreement in Slovak. Our experiments verify that the processes which specify the gender feature on past tense verbs are subject to interference from local nouns, and that the likelihood of…
Descriptors: Morphology (Languages), Memory, Foreign Countries, Sentences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dopkins, S.; Ngo, C.T. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2005
In studies of anaphor comprehension, the capacity for recognizing a noun in a sentence decreases following the resolution of a repeated-noun anaphor (Gernsbacher, 1989). In studies of recognition memory, the capacity for recognizing a noun in a scrambled sentence decreases following the recognition that another noun has occurred before in the…
Descriptors: Sentences, Nouns, Recognition (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Howell, S.R.; Jankowicz, D.; Becker, S. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2005
It is generally accepted that children have sensorimotor mental representations for concepts even before they learn the words for those concepts. We argue that these prelinguistic and embodied concepts direct and ground word learning, such that early concepts provide scaffolding by which later word learning, and even grammar learning, is enabled…
Descriptors: Nouns, Language Acquisition, Grammar
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mak, Willem M.; Vonk, Wietske; Schriefers, Herbert – Journal of Memory and Language, 2006
For several languages, a preference for subject relative clauses over object relative clauses has been reported. However, Mak, Vonk, and Schriefers (2002) showed that there is no such preference for relative clauses with an animate subject and an inanimate object. A Dutch object relative clause as...de rots, die de wandelaars beklommen hebben...…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Indo European Languages, Semantics, Nouns
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5