Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 29 |
Descriptor
Phonemes | 37 |
Phonology | 17 |
Language Processing | 15 |
Word Recognition | 12 |
Models | 10 |
Syllables | 10 |
Experiments | 9 |
Vowels | 8 |
Auditory Perception | 7 |
Cognitive Processes | 6 |
Memory | 6 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Memory and Language | 39 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 39 |
Reports - Research | 28 |
Reports - Evaluative | 8 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
McQueen, James M.; Jesse, Alexandra; Norris, Dennis – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
The strongest support for feedback in speech perception comes from evidence of apparent lexical influence on prelexical fricative-stop compensation for coarticulation. Lexical knowledge (e.g., that the ambiguous final fricative of "Christma?" should be [s]) apparently influences perception of following stops. We argue that all such previous…
Descriptors: Auditory Perception, Cognitive Psychology, Assistive Technology, Experiments
McMurray, Bob; Tanenhaus, Michael K.; Aslin, Richard N. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
Spoken word recognition shows gradient sensitivity to within-category voice onset time (VOT), as predicted by several current models of spoken word recognition, including TRACE (McClelland, J., & Elman, J. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. "Cognitive Psychology," 18, 1-86). It remains unclear, however, whether this sensitivity is…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Inhibition, Auditory Perception, Word Recognition
Arciuli, Joanne; Monaghan, Padraic; Seva, Nada – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
Models of reading aloud have tended to focus on the mapping between graphemes and phonemes in monosyllables. Critical adaptations of these models are required when considering the reading of polysyllables, which constitute over 90% of word types in English. In this paper, we examined one such adaptation--the process of stress assignment in…
Descriptors: Reading Materials, Cues, Investigations, Suprasegmentals
Lee, Yongeun; Goldrick, Matthew – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
In a variety of experimental paradigms speakers do not treat all sub-syllabic sequences equally. In languages like English, participants tend to group vowels and codas together to the exclusion of onsets (i.e., /bet/=/b/-/et/). Three possible accounts of these patterns are examined. A hierarchical account attributes these results to the presence…
Descriptors: Vowels, Short Term Memory, Language Processing, Phonemes
Oberauer, Klaus; Lange, Elke B. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
Reports two experiments on mechanism of interference in working memory. Experiment 1 shows that a target word in a memory list, which bears high similarity to one of 4 words read aloud in the retention interval, is recalled less well than a control word. A second target word, not similar to any word read aloud but with all its phonemes repeated…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Prediction, Memory, Verbal Communication
Acheson, Daniel J.; MacDonald, Maryellen C. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
Many accounts of working memory posit specialized storage mechanisms for the maintenance of serial order. We explore an alternative, that maintenance is achieved through temporary activation in the language production architecture. Four experiments examined the extent to which the phonological similarity effect can be explained as a sublexical…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Cognitive Processes, Experiments, Stimuli
Mani, Nivedita; Plunkett, Kim – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Infants become selectively sensitive to phonological distinctions relevant to their native language at an early age. One might expect that infants bring some of this phonological knowledge to bear in encoding the words they subsequently acquire. In line with this expectation, studies have found that 14-month-olds are sensitive to mispronunciations…
Descriptors: Infants, Vowels, Reading Skills, Phonemes
Jefferies, Elizabeth; Hoffman, Paul; Jones, Roy; Lambon Ralph, Matthew A. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
This study presents the first direct comparison of immediate serial recall in semantic dementia (SD) and transcortical sensory aphasia (TSA). Previous studies of the effect of semantic impairment on verbal short-term memory (STM) have led to important theoretical advances. However, different conclusions have been drawn from these two groups. This…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Phonology, Semantics, Dementia
Stemberger, Joseph Paul – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *"didn't broke" for target "didn't break") is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when…
Descriptors: Syntax, Rhyme, Morphemes, Error Patterns
Creel, Sarah C.; Aslin, Richard N.; Tanenhaus, Michael K. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2006
The role of segment similarity in early (i.e., partially learned) lexical entries was assessed using artificial lexicons in a referential context. During a learning phase participants heard 40 nonsense words, each accompanied by an unfamiliar picture. In testing, participants heard the direction ''Click on the [X]'', and chose which of four…
Descriptors: Experiments, Lexicology, Word Recognition, Syllables
Damian, Markus F.; Dumay, Nicolas – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Current accounts of spoken production debate the extent to which speakers plan ahead. Here, we investigated whether the scope of phonological planning is influenced by changes in time pressure constraints. The first experiment used a picture-word interference task and showed that picture naming latencies were shorter when word distractors shared…
Descriptors: Nouns, English (Second Language), Models, Phonology
Thiessen, Erik D. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Several recent experiments indicate that, when learning words, children are not as sensitive to phonemic differences (e.g., /d/ vs. /t/) as they are in discrimination tasks [Pater, J., Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. (2004). "The perceptual acquisition of phonological contrasts." "Language," 80, 384-402; Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. (1997).…
Descriptors: Phonemics, Young Children, Phonemes, Language Acquisition
Bailey, T.M.; Hahn, U. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2005
Similarity between component speech sounds influences language processing in numerous ways. Explanation and detailed prediction of linguistic performance consequently requires an understanding of these basic similarities. The research reported in this paper contrasts two broad classes of approach to the issue of phoneme similarity-theoretically…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Language Processing, Linguistic Performance
Mirman, D.; McClelland, J.L.; Holt, L.L. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2005
Previous studies have failed to demonstrate lexically induced delays in phoneme recognition, casting doubt on interactive models of speech perception. We present TRACE simulations that explain these failures: previously tested conditions failed to produce lexically induced delay effects because the input was too unambiguous and the control…
Descriptors: Prediction, Phonemes, Investigations, Competition
Kraljic, Tanya; Samuel, Arthur G. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Different speakers may pronounce the same sounds very differently, yet listeners have little difficulty perceiving speech accurately. Recent research suggests that listeners adjust their preexisting phonemic categories to accommodate speakers' pronunciations ("perceptual learning"). In some cases, these adjustments appear to reflect general…
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Auditory Perception, Phonemes, Cognitive Style