Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7 |
Descriptor
Models | 10 |
Phonemes | 9 |
Language Processing | 5 |
Phonology | 5 |
Word Recognition | 4 |
Syllables | 3 |
Alphabets | 2 |
Comparative Analysis | 2 |
Cues | 2 |
English | 2 |
Experiments | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Memory and Language | 10 |
Author
Andrews, Sally | 1 |
Arciuli, Joanne | 1 |
Arnon, Inbal | 1 |
Bond, Rachel | 1 |
Damian, Markus F. | 1 |
DePaolis, Rory A. | 1 |
Dell, Gary S. | 1 |
Dumay, Nicolas | 1 |
Dupoux, Emmanuel | 1 |
Frota, Sonia | 1 |
Halle, Pierre | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 10 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Dupoux, Emmanuel; Parlato, Erika; Frota, Sonia; Hirose, Yuki; Peperkamp, Sharon – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
Listeners of various languages tend to perceive an illusory vowel inside consonant clusters that are illegal in their native language. Here, we test whether this phenomenon arises after phoneme categorization or rather interacts with it. We assess the perception of illegal consonant clusters in native speakers of Japanese, Brazilian Portuguese,…
Descriptors: Cues, Vowels, Phonology, Classification
Arnon, Inbal; Snider, Neal – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
There is mounting evidence that language users are sensitive to distributional information at many grain-sizes. Much of this research has focused on the distributional properties of words, the units they consist of (morphemes, phonemes), and the syntactic structures they appear in (verb-categorization frames, syntactic constructions). In a series…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Form Classes (Languages), Morphemes, Language Processing
Lee, Chang H.; Taft, Marcus – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
When two consonants within an English word were transposed to create a nonword, difficulty in lexical decision responses to that nonword was revealed, most strongly when the coda of the first syllable was exchanged with the onset of the second (e.g., "nakpin" derived from "napkin"), but also when onsets were exchanged between syllables (e.g.,…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Semantics, Figurative Language, Syllables
Malins, Jeffrey G.; Joanisse, Marc F. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
We used eyetracking to examine how tonal versus segmental information influence spoken word recognition in Mandarin Chinese. Participants heard an auditory word and were required to identify its corresponding picture from an array that included the target item ("chuang2" "bed"), a phonological competitor (segmental: chuang1 "window"; cohort:…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Word Recognition, Mandarin Chinese, Language Processing
Nozari, Nazbanou; Kittredge, Audrey K.; Dell, Gary S.; Schwartz, Myrna F. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
This paper investigates the cognitive processes underlying picture naming and auditory word repetition. In the two-step model of lexical access, both the semantic and phonological steps are involved in naming, but the former has no role in repetition. Assuming recognition of the to-be-repeated word, repetition could consist of retrieving the…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Phonology, Semantics, Aphasia
Arciuli, Joanne; Monaghan, Padraic; Seva, Nada – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
Models of reading aloud have tended to focus on the mapping between graphemes and phonemes in monosyllables. Critical adaptations of these models are required when considering the reading of polysyllables, which constitute over 90% of word types in English. In this paper, we examined one such adaptation--the process of stress assignment in…
Descriptors: Reading Materials, Cues, Investigations, Suprasegmentals
Damian, Markus F.; Dumay, Nicolas – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Current accounts of spoken production debate the extent to which speakers plan ahead. Here, we investigated whether the scope of phonological planning is influenced by changes in time pressure constraints. The first experiment used a picture-word interference task and showed that picture naming latencies were shorter when word distractors shared…
Descriptors: Nouns, English (Second Language), Models, Phonology
Andrews, Sally; Woollams, Anna; Bond, Rachel – Journal of Memory and Language, 2005
Two experiments investigated naming performance for items with and without digraphs. Both experiments compared performance for Regular Consistent, Regular Inconsistent and Exception words. Experiment 1 also compared nonwords with Non-Existent Bodies to those with existing Consistent and Inconsistent Bodies. Naming was slower for nonwords…
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Word Recognition, Phoneme Grapheme Correspondence, Graphemes
Vihman, Marilyn M.; Nakai, Satsuki; DePaolis, Rory A.; Halle, Pierre – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
The interaction between prosodic and segmental aspects of infant representations for speech was explored using the head-turn paradigm, with untrained everyday familiar words and phrases as stimuli. At 11 months English-learning infants, like French infants (Halle & Boysson-Bardies, 1994), attended significantly longer to a list of familiar lexical…
Descriptors: Infants, Word Recognition, Models, Suprasegmentals
Perea, Manuel; Lupker, Stephen J. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
Nonwords created by transposing two "adjacent" letters (i.e., transposed-letter (TL) nonwords like "jugde") are very effective at activating the lexical representation of their base words. This fact poses problems for most computational models of word recognition (e.g., the interactive-activation model and its extensions), which assume that exact…
Descriptors: Alphabets, Word Recognition, Models, Lexicology