NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 8 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Georgiou, George K.; Parrila, Rauno; Cui, Ying; Papadopoulos, Timothy C. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
The objective of this study was to examine why rapid automatized naming (RAN) is related to reading by manipulating processes involved at the input, processing, and output stages of its production. In total, 65 children in Grade 2 and 65 in Grade 6 were assessed on serial and discrete RAN (Digits and Objects), Cancellation, RAN Yes/No, and oral…
Descriptors: Reading Fluency, Grade 2, Grade 6, Naming
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kim, Young-Suk; Wagner, Richard K.; Lopez, Danielle – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
From a developmental framework, relations among list reading fluency, oral and silent reading fluency, listening comprehension, and reading comprehension might be expected to change as children's reading skills develop. We examined developmental relations among these constructs in a latent-variable longitudinal study of first and second graders.…
Descriptors: Reading Comprehension, Listening Comprehension, Silent Reading, Reading Fluency
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
de Jong, Peter F.; Bitter, Danielle J. L.; van Setten, Margot; Marinus, Eva – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
Two studies were conducted to test the central claim of the self-teaching hypothesis (i.e., phonological recoding is necessary for orthographic learning) in silent reading. The first study aimed to demonstrate the use of phonological recoding during silent reading. Texts containing pseudowords were read silently or aloud. Two days later, target…
Descriptors: Silent Reading, Phonology, Reading, Spelling
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Alario, F.-Xavier; De Cara, Bruno; Ziegler, Johannes C. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2007
The picture-word interference paradigm was used to shed new light on the debate concerning slow serial versus fast parallel activation of phonology in silent reading. Prereaders, beginning readers (Grades 1-4), and adults named pictures that had words printed on them. Words and pictures shared phonology either at the beginnings of words (e.g.,…
Descriptors: Phonology, Silent Reading, Beginning Reading, Elementary School Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
McCutchen, Deborah; Crain-Thoreson, Catherine – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1994
Two experiments studied the role of phonemic information in children's comprehension during silent reading. A sentence acceptability task indicated that readers required more time to read and comprehend sentences with word-initial phonemes (the "tongue-twister effect") than control sentences. When the first task was added to a digit…
Descriptors: Phonemic Awareness, Preadolescents, Reading Comprehension, Reading Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bowey, Judith A.; Muller, David – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2005
This study examined rapid orthographic learning following silent reading in third-grade children as a function of number of target nonword repetitions and test delay. In each of two test sessions at least 6 days apart, children read a series of short stories, with each story containing a different nonword repeated either four or eight times. In…
Descriptors: Silent Reading, Reading Processes, Phonology, Phonetic Transcription
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Sprenger-Charolles, Liliane; Siegel, Linda S.; Bechennec, Danielle; Serniclaes, Willy – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2003
Development of children's phonological and orthographic processing was studied from middle of grade 1 to end of grade 4. Signs of reliance on phonological processing were found on reading aloud, spelling, and silent reading tasks, even with indicators of reliance on orthographic processing. Phonological and orthographic processing appeared to be…
Descriptors: Child Development, Children, Elementary School Students, Learning Strategies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Beggs, W. D. A.; Howarth, Philippa N. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1985
Suggests that inner speech is a manifestation of the need to prestructure oral utterances. Among the results, inner speech was found to be acquired by normally developing readers between the ages of 8 and 11, and children comprehended text better when certain prosodic features were made visible on the text. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Elementary Education, Elementary School Students, Inner Speech (Subvocal), Oral Reading