Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 3 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 8 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 62 |
Descriptor
Language Research | 439 |
Child Language | 387 |
Language Acquisition | 363 |
Psycholinguistics | 138 |
Young Children | 115 |
Vocabulary Development | 69 |
Language Processing | 68 |
Infants | 67 |
Semantics | 67 |
Syntax | 61 |
Verbs | 59 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Child Language | 439 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 363 |
Reports - Research | 315 |
Reports - Evaluative | 16 |
Opinion Papers | 15 |
Information Analyses | 14 |
Reports - Descriptive | 13 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 4 |
Reports - General | 2 |
Reference Materials -… | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 14 |
Preschool Education | 7 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 3 |
Elementary Education | 2 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
United Kingdom | 3 |
Australia | 2 |
Canada | 2 |
Netherlands | 2 |
Norway | 2 |
Austria | 1 |
China (Beijing) | 1 |
Denmark | 1 |
France | 1 |
Germany | 1 |
Hong Kong | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
MacArthur Communicative… | 4 |
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Does not meet standards | 1 |

Lanza, Elizabeth – Journal of Child Language, 1992
This study applies perspectives from sociolinguistics to investigate the language mixing of a bilingual two year old acquiring Norwegian and English simultaneously in Norway. The investigation stresses the need to examine more carefully the roles of dominance and context in the language mixing of young bilingual children. (40 references) (JL)
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Child Language, Code Switching (Language), English

Marchman, Virginia A.; Bates, Elizabeth – Journal of Child Language, 1994
This paper outlines the degree to which age and verb vocabulary size are predictive of changes in the reported usage of English verbs that are irregular in their past tense form in a sample of more than 1,000 children. (Contains 40 references.) (JL)
Descriptors: Age, Child Language, English, Language Acquisition

Reznick, J. Steven; Goldfield, Beverly A. – Journal of Child Language, 1994
Infants were followed longitudinally from 1;2 to 1;10. Parents maintained a journal of child's spoken words and, at 2-month intervals, completed representative checklist of words produced. Results suggest that the diary method is more effective during early emergence of language, and the representative checklist method is more effective late in…
Descriptors: Check Lists, Child Language, Comparative Analysis, Diaries

Barrett, Martyn; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1991
Followup to earlier report that focused on initial uses of first 10 words produced by 4 children is presented. Results of analysis of subsequent use of these 40 words is presented. Findings indicate that the role of linguistic input in early lexical development may decline sharply once a child has established initial uses for words. (24…
Descriptors: Caregiver Speech, Comparative Analysis, Infants, Language Patterns

Ahktar, Nameera; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1991
Twelve mothers were videotaped interacting with their children, aged 13 months, and 100 maternal utterances were coded for pragmatic intent. Prescriptives were coded as either changing or following the child's focus of attention. Results show that, given a joint focus, directing a 13-month-old's behavior can have beneficial effects on subsequent…
Descriptors: Behavior Modification, Coding, Infants, Language Acquisition

Dinnsen, Daniel A.; Barlow, Jessica A. – Journal of Child Language, 1998
Drawing on archival data on the sound systems of five children ages 3 to 4 with normal development and 47 children ages 3 to 6 with phonological delay, one chain shift (interaction of phonological substitution errors) was identified in the speech of six children. Different derivational and constraint-based accounts of the chain shift were…
Descriptors: Child Language, Language Acquisition, Language Processing, Language Research

Graham, Susan A.; Poulin-Dubois, Diane – Journal of Child Language, 1999
Two experiments examined infants' reliance on object shape versus color for word generalization to animate and inanimate objects. Infants were taught labels for either novel vehicles or novel animals using preferential-looking procedure or an interactive procedure. Results of both experiments indicated that infants limited their word…
Descriptors: Animals, Auditory Stimuli, Child Language, Color
Hurtado, Nereyda; Marchman, Virginia A.; Fernald, Anne – Journal of Child Language, 2007
Research on the development of efficiency in spoken language understanding has focused largely on middle-class children learning English. Here we extend this research to Spanish-learning children (n=49; M=2;0; range=1;3-3;1) living in the USA in Latino families from primarily low socioeconomic backgrounds. Children looked at pictures of familiar…
Descriptors: Language Research, Eye Movements, Oral Language, Disadvantaged Youth

Savic, Svenka – Journal of Child Language, 1975
The early acquisition of the interrogative system, with data from Serbo-Croatian, is investigated. The subject is approached from the angle of adult-child interaction. A first-born pair of dizygotic twins were observed, beginning a month prior to the time when they first began to produce questions. (Author/RM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Infant Behavior, Language Acquisition, Language Patterns

Martlew, Margaret; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1978
To explore the relationship of language use and speech adaptation to role and context, the spontaneous speech of a 5-year-old boy was recorded in three different situations: playing alone, playing with a friend of the same age, and playing with his mother. (Author/SW)
Descriptors: Child Language, Language Acquisition, Language Research, Language Styles

French, Lucia A.; Brown, Ann L. – Journal of Child Language, 1977
Preschool children were required to act out a series of two-event sequences conjoined by either "before" or "after." Performance was markedly superior for meaningfully ordered sequences than for arbitrarily ordered sequences. It is suggested that the meanings of "before" and "after" must be acquired in situations which provide contextual support.…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Comprehension, Concept Formation

Homzie, M. J.; Gravitt, Carol B. – Journal of Child Language, 1977
In retelling 20 stories, 23 nursery-school children often refused to produce sentences in which causation was stated directly, but readily retold causation-implied utterances. Other results are discussed. (Author/RM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation, Language Acquisition

Perner, Josef; Leekam, Susan R. – Journal of Child Language, 1986
Investigates young children's ability to adjust the content of their verbal responses according to what they know their listener knows. Younger and older three-year-old children were able to discern what another person knew and did not know and adjusted their responses accordingly. Younger three-year-olds tended to be underinformative. (SED)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Language, Cognitive Development, Communication Skills

O'Grady, William; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1986
Tests the prediction that children acquiring left-branching languages will exhibit a preference for backward patterns of anaphora by presenting data from Japanese and Korean which show the prediction to be false. Findings support the view that any directionality preference for anaphora is the same for all languages. (Author/SED)
Descriptors: Child Language, Children, Generative Grammar, Interviews

Camarata, Stephen; Lennard, Laurence B. – Journal of Child Language, 1986
Describes a study of young children's production of novel words serving as names of objects and actions, which were matched according to consonant and syllable structure. On each measure, accuarate production of new consonants was greater for the object words, possibly because action words have greater semantic complexity than object words. (SED)
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Child Language, Comprehension, Consonants