Publication Date
In 2025 | 7 |
Since 2024 | 48 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 155 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 312 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 734 |
Descriptor
Source
Developmental Science | 801 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 801 |
Reports - Research | 625 |
Reports - Evaluative | 145 |
Reports - Descriptive | 24 |
Information Analyses | 6 |
Opinion Papers | 4 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 81 |
Elementary Education | 46 |
Preschool Education | 18 |
Primary Education | 17 |
Kindergarten | 12 |
Grade 1 | 9 |
Grade 3 | 8 |
Grade 4 | 7 |
Adult Education | 6 |
Grade 2 | 6 |
Higher Education | 5 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Location
Germany | 9 |
China | 8 |
Japan | 8 |
United Kingdom | 6 |
Netherlands | 5 |
Romania | 4 |
Australia | 3 |
Bangladesh | 3 |
Bolivia | 3 |
Ghana | 3 |
Italy | 3 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Does not meet standards | 1 |
Doherty, Martin J.; Campbell, Nicola M.; Tsuji, Hiromi; Phillips, William A. – Developmental Science, 2010
The sensitivity of size perception to context has been used to distinguish between "vision for action" and "vision for perception", and to study cultural, psychopathological, and developmental differences in perception. The status of that evidence is much debated, however. Here we use a rigorous double dissociation paradigm based on the Ebbinghaus…
Descriptors: Vision, Young Children, Pathology, Age Differences
Shinskey, Jeanne L.; Munakata, Yuko – Developmental Science, 2010
Novelty seeking is viewed as adaptive, and novelty preferences in infancy predict cognitive performance into adulthood. Yet 7-month-olds prefer familiar stimuli to novel ones when searching for hidden objects, in contrast to their strong novelty preferences with visible objects (Shinskey & Munakata, 2005). According to a graded representations…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Stimuli, Familiarity, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Steenbeek, Henderien; van Geert, Paul – Developmental Science, 2008
Studying short-term dynamic processes and change mechanisms in interaction yields important knowledge that contributes to understanding long-term social development of children. In order to get a grip on this short-term dynamics of interaction processes, the authors made a dynamic systems model of dyadic interaction of children during one play…
Descriptors: Play, Peer Relationship, Interaction, Social Development
Leo, Irene; Simion, Francesca – Developmental Science, 2009
The present study was aimed at exploring newborns' ability to recognize configural changes within real face images by testing newborns' sensitivity to the Thatcher illusion. Using the habituation procedure, newborns' ability to discriminate between an unaltered face image and the same face with the eyes and the mouth 180 degrees rotated (i.e.…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Neonates, Spatial Ability, Habituation
Marcovitch, Stuart; Zelazo, Philip David – Developmental Science, 2009
The hierarchical competing systems model (HCSM) provides a framework for understanding the emergence and early development of executive function--the cognitive processes underlying the conscious control of behavior--in the context of search for hidden objects. According to this model, behavior is determined by the joint influence of a…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Cognitive Processes, Models, Child Development
Bulloch, Megan J.; Opfer, John E. – Developmental Science, 2009
Development of reasoning is often depicted as involving increasing use of relational similarities and decreasing use of perceptual similarities ("the perceptual-to-relational shift"). We argue that this shift is a special case of a broader developmental trend: increasing sensitivity to the predictive accuracy of different similarity types. To test…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Abstract Reasoning, Hypothesis Testing, Classification
Corriveau, Kathleen; Harris, Paul L. – Developmental Science, 2009
To determine whether children retain a preference for a previously accurate informant only in the short term or for long-term use, 3- and 4-year-old children were tested in two experiments. In both experiments, children were given accuracy information about two informants and were subsequently tested for their selective trust in the two informants…
Descriptors: Trust (Psychology), Age Differences, Preschool Children, Child Development
Wang, Su-hua; Mitroff, Stephen R. – Developmental Science, 2009
Combining theoretical hypotheses of infant cognition and adult perception, we present evidence that infants can maintain visual representations despite their failure to detect a change. Infants under 12 months typically fail to notice a change to an object's height in a covering event. The present experiments demonstrated that 11-month-old infants…
Descriptors: Infants, Visual Perception, Visual Stimuli, Cognitive Processes
Van Herwegen, Jo; Ansari, Daniel; Xu, Fei; Karmiloff-Smith, Annette – Developmental Science, 2008
Previous studies have suggested that typically developing 6-month-old infants are able to discriminate between small and large numerosities. However, discrimination between small numerosities in young infants is only possible when variables continuous with number (e.g. area or circumference) are confounded. In contrast, large number discrimination…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Infants, Number Concepts, Numeracy
Balas, Benjamin – Developmental Science, 2010
Newborn infants appear to possess an innate bias that guides preferential orienting to and tracking of human faces. There is, however, no clear agreement as to the underlying mechanism supporting such a preference. In particular, two competing theories (known as the "structural" and "sensory" hypotheses) conjecture fundamentally different biasing…
Descriptors: Investigations, Infants, Human Body, Psychomotor Skills
Libertus, Melissa E.; Brannon, Elizabeth M. – Developmental Science, 2010
Previous studies have shown that as a group 6-month-old infants successfully discriminate numerical changes when the values differ by at least a 1:2 ratio but fail at a 2:3 ratio (e.g. 8 vs. 16 but not 8 vs. 12). However, no studies have yet examined individual differences in number discrimination in infancy. Using a novel numerical change…
Descriptors: Infants, Visual Stimuli, Visual Discrimination, Numbers
Farah, Martha J.; Betancourt, Laura; Shera, David M.; Savage, Jessica H.; Giannetta, Joan M.; Brodsky, Nancy L.; Malmud, Elsa K.; Hurt, Hallam – Developmental Science, 2008
The effects of environmental stimulation and parental nurturance on brain development have been studied extensively in animals. Much less is known about the relations between childhood experience and cognitive development in humans. Using a longitudinally collected data set with ecologically valid in-home measures of childhood experience and later…
Descriptors: Stimulation, Children, Brain, Language Acquisition
Dilks, Daniel D.; Hoffman, James E.; Landau, Barbara – Developmental Science, 2008
Evidence suggests that visual processing is divided into the dorsal ("how") and ventral ("what") streams. We examined the normal development of these streams and their breakdown under neurological deficit by comparing performance of normally developing children and Williams syndrome individuals on two tasks: a visually guided action ("how") task,…
Descriptors: Vision, Cognitive Processes, Child Development, Developmental Stages
Perry, Lynn K.; Smith, Linda B.; Hockema, Stephen A. – Developmental Science, 2008
Recent research has shown that 2-year-olds fail at a task that ostensibly only requires the ability to understand that solid objects cannot pass through other solid objects. Two experiments were conducted in which 2- and 3-year-olds judged the stopping point of an object as it moved at varying speeds along a path and behind an occluder, stopping…
Descriptors: Young Children, Cognitive Development, Motion, Child Development
Poti, Patrizia; Hayashi, Misato; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro – Developmental Science, 2009
Spatial construction tasks are basic tests of visual-spatial processing. Two studies have assessed spatial construction skills in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and young children (Homo sapiens sapiens) with a block modelling task. Study 1a subjects were three young chimpanzees and five adult chimpanzees. Study 1b subjects were 30 human children…
Descriptors: Animals, Primatology, Spatial Ability, Young Children