NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 22 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bielak, Allison A. M.; Anstey, Kaarin J. – Developmental Psychology, 2019
Intraindividual variability (IIV) in cognitive speed, or moment-to-moment changes in ability, is a developmental phenomenon indicative of neurological integrity that increases gradually across adulthood. Past research has shown that IIV negatively covaries with cognitive performance, in which higher IIV at one occasion is associated with poorer…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Individual Differences, Cognitive Ability, Adult Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rast, Philippe – Developmental Psychology, 2011
The present study aimed at modeling individual differences in a verbal learning task by means of a latent structured growth curve approach based on an exponential function that yielded 3 parameters: initial recall, learning rate, and asymptotic performance. Three cognitive variables--speed of information processing, verbal knowledge, working…
Descriptors: Verbal Learning, Predictor Variables, Individual Differences, Short Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Rickard, Henry C.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1970
Descriptors: Imitation, Kindergarten Children, Verbal Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Moore, Chris; Angelopoulos, Maria; Bennett, Paula – Developmental Psychology, 1999
This study investigated novel word acquisition by 18- and 24-month-old children in the context of adult referential behavior independent of variations in salience. Findings suggest that 24-month olds use referential intent of the speaker to learn new words, but when learning, they may have a less secure grasp on the meaning of speakers'…
Descriptors: Cues, Language Acquisition, Toddlers, Verbal Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Conrad, R. – Developmental Psychology, 1971
Results of an experiment with children ages 3-11 years performing serial recall tasks suggest that it is not until about age 5 years that children's overt speech reaches a functional stage that would justify internalization. (Author/WY)
Descriptors: Child Development, Inner Speech (Subvocal), Memorization, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fraunfelker, Barbara S. – Developmental Psychology, 1971
Studies the effect of phonetic compatibility upon verbal learning and concludes that it is a task variable of considerable magnitude. (WY)
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Grade 1, Grade 3, Paired Associate Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Diesendruck, Gil – Developmental Psychology, 2005
In Study 1, 4-year-olds avoided 2 names for an object when exposed to a common or a proper noun in a puppet's presence or to a common noun in a puppet's absence, but not when exposed to a proper noun in a puppet's absence. In Study 2, 3-year-olds avoided 2 names for an object when the requester for the referent of a second label in a different…
Descriptors: Puppetry, Nouns, Monolingualism, Bilingualism
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Smith, Anderson D. – Developmental Psychology, 1976
The results of this study indicate that age differences in the recall of high frequency word lists are not related to the total presentation time of the lists but appear to be affected by differences in retrieval processes as a function of age. (JMB)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Memory, Recall (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Cann, Linda F.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1973
This demonstration of release from proactive interference with young children confirms the suggestion that the technique is appropriate for the study of developmental changes in the encoding of information. (Authors/CB)
Descriptors: Black Students, Cognitive Processes, Elementary School Students, Interference (Language)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1992
In three experiments, adults and 30-month-old children (1) selected a novel object as the referent for a novel term; (2) extended the new word to another exemplar; and (3) allowed the new word to preempt another novel label from applying to the just-named object. (BC)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Classification, Comparative Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Kossuth, Gina L.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1971
Descriptors: Cluster Grouping, Grade 6, Memory, Middle Class
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Al-Namlah, Abdulrahman S.; Fernyhough, Charles; Meins, Elizabeth – Developmental Psychology, 2006
Cross-national stability in private speech (PS) and short-term memory was investigated in Saudi Arabian (n=63) and British (n=58) 4- to 8-year-olds. Assumed differences in child-adult interaction between the 2 nationality groups led to predictions of Gender ? Nationality interactions in the development of verbal mediation. British boys used more…
Descriptors: Sociocultural Patterns, Gender Differences, Cognitive Processes, Foreign Countries
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Katz, Phyllis A.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1971
Descriptors: Age Differences, Associative Learning, Cues, Grade 1
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Paris, Scott G. – Developmental Psychology, 1978
Presents a study of age differences in memory organization during repeated recall tasks. Second and sixth grade children served as subjects. (BD)
Descriptors: Advance Organizers, Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Elementary School Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
McGeehan, Deborah L.; Cantor, Joan H. – Developmental Psychology, 1977
In a combined verbal and motor paired-associate task, children in kindergarten, second grade, and fourth grade learned nonsense names and spatial responses for three random shapes. Results showed evidence for mediation at all three age levels and increased mediational effects with age. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students, Mediation Theory
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2