NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Source
Developmental Psychology638
Audience
Researchers35
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Elementary and Secondary…1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Does not meet standards1
Showing 181 to 195 of 638 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Beier, Jonathan S.; Over, Harriet; Carpenter, Malinda – Developmental Psychology, 2014
From early in development, humans have strong prosocial tendencies. Much research has documented young children's propensity to help others achieve their unfulfilled goals toward physical objects. Yet many of our most common and important goals are social--directed toward other people. Here we demonstrate that children are also inclined, and able,…
Descriptors: Young Children, Control Groups, Goal Orientation, Prosocial Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Montroy, Janelle J.; Bowles, Ryan P.; Skibbe, Lori E.; McClelland, Megan M.; Morrison, Frederick J. – Developmental Psychology, 2016
The development of early childhood self-regulation is often considered an early life marker for later life successes. Yet little longitudinal research has evaluated whether there are different trajectories of self-regulation development across children. This study investigates the development of behavioral self-regulation between the ages of 3 and…
Descriptors: Self Control, Young Children, Child Development, Preschool Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rodgers, Rachel F.; Damiano, Stephanie R.; Wertheim, Eleanor H.; Paxton, Susan J. – Developmental Psychology, 2017
Media exposure among young children has been suggested to influence self-concept and the adoption of social stereotypes regarding body weight, as well as being associated with increased weight. The aim of this study was to examine the role of TV/DVD viewing in the development of positive stereotypes toward thinness, self-esteem and body mass index…
Descriptors: Mass Media Effects, Young Children, Females, Body Composition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Song, Ruiting; Over, Harriet; Carpenter, Malinda – Developmental Psychology, 2015
Humans have a strong need to belong. Thus, when signs of ostracism are detected, adults often feel motivated to affiliate with others in order to reestablish their social connections. This study investigated the importance of affiliation to young children following priming with ostracism. Four- and 5-year-old children were primed with either…
Descriptors: Young Children, Freehand Drawing, Priming, Rejection (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Denison, Stephanie; Reed, Christie; Xu, Fei – Developmental Psychology, 2013
How do people make rich inferences from such sparse data? Recent research has explored this inferential ability by investigating probabilistic reasoning in infancy. For example, 8- and 11-month-old infants can make inferences from samples to populations and vice versa (Denison & Xu, 2010a; Xu & Denison, 2009; Xu & Garcia, 2008a). The…
Descriptors: Probability, Infants, Inferences, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ronfard, Samuel; Harris, Paul L. – Developmental Psychology, 2014
As children listen to a simple action-based narrative, they construct a dynamic representation of the protagonist's movements, visual perspective, and goal-directed thoughts. We examined children's representations of more complex narratives in which the protagonist will encounter an unexpected outcome upon reaching his or her goal. Three studies…
Descriptors: Young Children, Preschool Children, Theory of Mind, Attribution Theory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cohen, Dale J.; Sarnecka, Barbara W. – Developmental Psychology, 2014
Children's understanding of numbers is often assessed using a number-line task, where the child is shown a line labeled with 0 at one end and a higher number (e.g., 100) at the other end. The child is then asked where on the line some intermediate number (e.g., 70) should go. Performance on this task changes predictably during childhood, and this…
Descriptors: Number Concepts, Computation, Measurement, Mathematics Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Yamashiro, Amy; Vouloumanos, Athena – Developmental Psychology, 2019
Adult humans process communicative interactions by recognizing that information is being communicated through speech (linguistic ability) and simultaneously evaluating how to respond appropriately (social-pragmatic ability). These abilities may originate in infancy. Infants understand how speech communicates in social interactions, helping them…
Descriptors: Pragmatics, Interpersonal Competence, Speech Communication, Autism
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Xu, Chang; LeFevre, Jo-Anne – Developmental Psychology, 2016
Are there differential benefits of training sequential number knowledge versus spatial skills for children's numerical and spatial performance? Three- to five-year-old children (N = 84) participated in 1 session of either sequential training (e.g., what comes before and after the number 5?) or non-numerical spatial training (i.e., decomposition of…
Descriptors: Young Children, Preschool Children, Numbers, Mathematics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Chen, Zhe; Honomichl, Ryan; Kennedy, Diane; Tan, Enda – Developmental Psychology, 2016
The present study examines 5- to 8-year-old children's relation reasoning in solving matrix completion tasks. This study incorporates a componential analysis, an eye-tracking method, and a microgenetic approach, which together allow an investigation of the cognitive processing strategies involved in the development and learning of children's…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Statistical Analysis, Componential Analysis, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sawyer, Alyssa C. P.; Miller-Lewis, Lauren R.; Searle, Amelia K.; Sawyer, Michael G.; Lynch, John W. – Developmental Psychology, 2015
The aim of this study was to determine whether the extent of improvement in self-regulation achieved between ages 4 and 6 years is associated with the level of behavioral problems later in childhood. Participants were 4-year-old children (n = 510) attending preschools in South Australia. Children's level of self-regulation was assessed using the…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Self Control, Improvement, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Maldarelli, Jennifer E.; Kahrs, Björn A.; Hunt, Sarah C.; Lockman, Jeffrey J. – Developmental Psychology, 2015
Despite the importance of handwriting for school readiness and early academic progress, prior research on the development of handwriting has focused primarily on the product rather than the process by which young children write letters. In contrast, in the present work, early handwriting is viewed as involving a suite of perceptual, motor, and…
Descriptors: Handwriting, Young Children, Visual Perception, Psychomotor Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Austin, Keith; Theakston, Anna; Lieven, Elena; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Psychology, 2014
Although a fair amount is known about young children's production of negation, little is known about their comprehension. Here, we focus on arguably the most complex basic form, denial, and how young children understand denial, when it is expressed in response to a question with gesture, single word, or sentence. One hundred twenty-six children in…
Descriptors: Young Children, Comprehension, Defense Mechanisms, Nonverbal Communication
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Martin, Alia; Olson, Kristina R. – Developmental Psychology, 2013
Helping others is often more complicated than fulfilling their requests, for instance, when an individual requests something that is not suited to achieving her or his ultimate goal. Are children indiscriminate helpers, responding to any object-directed action or request, or do their helping actions prioritize ultimate goals over specific…
Descriptors: Helping Relationship, Young Children, Child Behavior, Prosocial Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Waller, Rebecca; Dishion, Thomas J.; Shaw, Daniel S.; Gardner, Frances; Wilson, Melvin N.; Hyde, Luke W. – Developmental Psychology, 2016
Callous-unemotional (CU) behavior has been linked to behavior problems in children and adolescents. However, few studies have examined whether CU behavior in "early childhood" predicts behavior problems or CU behavior in "late childhood". This study examined whether indicators of CU behavior at ages 2-4 predicted aggression,…
Descriptors: Emotional Response, Child Behavior, Behavior Problems, Children
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  9  |  10  |  11  |  12  |  13  |  14  |  15  |  16  |  17  |  ...  |  43