Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Investigations | 8 |
Task Analysis | 8 |
Cognitive Processes | 7 |
Cues | 3 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Attention | 2 |
Children | 2 |
Cognitive Development | 2 |
Cognitive Mapping | 2 |
Comparative Analysis | 2 |
Evidence | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognition | 8 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 8 |
Reports - Research | 5 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Adult Education | 2 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Evans, Theodore A.; Beran, Michael J. – Cognition, 2012
Prospective memory (PM) involves forming intentions, retaining those intentions, and later executing those intended responses at the appropriate time. Few studies have investigated this capacity in animals. Monkeys performed a computerized task that assessed their ability to remember to make a particular response if they observed a PM cue embedded…
Descriptors: Memory, Stimuli, Intention, Investigations
Capizzi, Mariagrazia; Sanabria, Daniel; Correa, Angel – Cognition, 2012
The aim of the present study was to investigate the controlled versus the automatic nature of temporal preparation. If temporal preparation involves controlled rather than automatic processing, it should be reduced by the addition of a concurrent demanding task. This hypothesis was tested by comparing participants' performance in a temporal…
Descriptors: Evidence, Cues, Short Term Memory, Cognitive Processes
De Bruin, L. C.; Newen, A. – Cognition, 2012
The elicited-response false belief task has traditionally been considered as reliably indicating that children acquire an understanding of false belief around 4 years of age. However, recent investigations using spontaneous-response tasks suggest that false belief understanding emerges much earlier. This leads to a developmental paradox: if young…
Descriptors: Investigations, Preschool Children, Infants, Organizations (Groups)
Gebuis, Titia; Gevers, Wim – Cognition, 2011
de Hevia and Spelke (de Hevia and Spelke (2009). Spontaneous mapping of number and space in adults and young children, "Cognition, 110", 198-207) investigated the mapping of number onto space. To this end, they introduced a non-symbolic flanker task. Here subjects have to bisect a line that is flanked by a 2-dot and a 9-dot array. Similar to the…
Descriptors: Cues, Cognitive Processes, Investigations, Cognitive Mapping
Berteletti, Ilaria; Lucangeli, Daniela; Zorzi, Marco – Cognition, 2012
The representation of numerical and non-numerical ordered sequences was investigated in children from preschool to grade 3. The child's conception of how sequence items map onto a spatial scale was tested using the Number-to-Position task (Siegler & Opfer, 2003) and new variants of the task designed to probe the representation of the alphabet…
Descriptors: Grade 3, Investigations, Preschool Education, Task Analysis
Thomas, Laura E.; Seiffert, Adriane E. – Cognition, 2010
Investigations of multiple-object tracking aim to further our understanding of how people perform common activities such as driving in traffic. However, tracking tasks in the laboratory have overlooked a crucial component of much real-world object tracking: self-motion. We investigated the hypothesis that keeping track of one's own movement…
Descriptors: Investigations, Attention, Motion, Task Analysis
Cheyne, James Allan; Carriere, Jonathan S. A.; Solman, Grayden J. F.; Smilek, Daniel – Cognition, 2011
Attention lapses resulting from reactivity to task challenges and their consequences constitute a pervasive factor affecting everyday performance errors and accidents. A bidirectional model of attention lapses (error [image omitted] attention-lapse: Cheyne, Solman, Carriere, & Smilek, 2009) argues that errors beget errors by generating attention…
Descriptors: Accidents, Learning Processes, Educational Environment, Partnerships in Education
Weiermann, Brigitte; Meier, Beat – Cognition, 2012
The purpose of the present study was to investigate incidental sequence learning across the lifespan. We tested 50 children (aged 7-16), 50 young adults (aged 20-30), and 50 older adults (aged >65) with a sequence learning paradigm that involved both a task and a response sequence. After several blocks of practice, all age groups slowed down…
Descriptors: Evidence, Older Adults, Young Adults, Learning Processes