Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 23 |
Descriptor
Task Analysis | 23 |
Experiments | 22 |
Cognitive Processes | 12 |
Attention | 6 |
Auditory Stimuli | 5 |
Models | 5 |
Cognitive Development | 4 |
Evaluation Methods | 4 |
Evidence | 4 |
Interaction | 4 |
Observation | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognition | 23 |
Author
Parmentier, Fabrice B. R. | 2 |
Apperly, Ian A. | 1 |
Back, Elisa | 1 |
Blank, Hartmut | 1 |
Bogartz, Richard S. | 1 |
Brosch, Tobias | 1 |
Brunye, Tad T. | 1 |
Campanella, Fabio | 1 |
Carreiras, Manuel | 1 |
Cheyne, J. Allan | 1 |
Chu, Yun | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 23 |
Reports - Research | 15 |
Reports - Evaluative | 8 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Solman, Grayden J. F.; Cheyne, J. Allan; Smilek, Daniel – Cognition, 2012
We present results from five search experiments using a novel "unpacking" paradigm in which participants use a mouse to sort through random heaps of distractors to locate the target. We report that during this task participants often fail to recognize the target despite moving it, and despite having looked at the item. Additionally, the missed…
Descriptors: Evidence, Experiments, Models, Computer Peripherals
Zultan, Ro'i; Gerstenberg, Tobias; Lagnado, David A. – Cognition, 2012
Attributions of responsibility play a critical role in many group interactions. This paper explores the role of causal and counterfactual reasoning in blame attributions in groups. We develop a general framework that builds on the notion of pivotality: an agent is pivotal if she could have changed the group outcome by acting differently. In three…
Descriptors: Cooperative Learning, Interaction, Experiments, Task Analysis
Pincham, Hannah L.; Szucs, Denes – Cognition, 2012
Subitizing is traditionally described as the rapid, preattentive and automatic enumeration of up to four items. Counting, by contrast, describes the enumeration of larger sets of items and requires slower serial shifts of attention. Although recent research has called into question the preattentive nature of subitizing, whether or not numerosities…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Attention, Computation, Visual Stimuli
Magnani, Barbara; Pavani, Francesco; Frassinetti, Francesca – Cognition, 2012
The aim of the present study was to explore the spatial organization of auditory time and the effects of the manipulation of spatial attention on such a representation. In two experiments, we asked 28 adults to classify the duration of auditory stimuli as "short" or "long". Stimuli were tones of high or low pitch, delivered left or right of the…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Auditory Stimuli, Attention, Experiments
Oeberst, Aileen; Blank, Hartmut – Cognition, 2012
Presenting inconsistent postevent information about a witnessed incident typically decreases the accuracy of memory reports concerning that event (the "misinformation effect"). Surprisingly, the "reversibility" of the effect (after an initial occurrence) has remained largely unexplored. Based on a "memory conversion" theoretical framework and…
Descriptors: Recall (Psychology), Memory, Models, Experiments
Bogartz, Richard S.; Staub, Adrian – Cognition, 2012
In three experimental tasks Stephen and Mirman (2010) measured gaze steps, the distance in pixels between gaze positions on successive samples from an eyetracker. They argued that the distribution of gaze steps is best fit by the lognormal distribution, and based on this analysis they concluded that interactive cognitive processes underlie eye…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Cognitive Processes, Experiments, Task Analysis
Davidenko, Nicolas; Flusberg, Stephen J. – Cognition, 2012
Visual processing is highly sensitive to stimulus orientation; for example, face perception is drastically worse when faces are oriented inverted vs. upright. However, stimulus orientation must be established in relation to a particular reference frame, and in most studies, several reference frames are conflated. Which reference frame(s) matter in…
Descriptors: Recognition (Psychology), Task Analysis, Experiments, Perception
Brosch, Tobias; Van Bavel, Jay J. – Cognition, 2012
There is extensive evidence that emotional--especially threatening--stimuli rapidly capture attention. These findings are often explained in terms of a hard-wired and relatively inflexible fear module. We propose an alternative, more flexible mechanism, arguing that motivational relevance is the crucial factor driving rapid attentional orienting.…
Descriptors: Evidence, Individual Differences, Cues, Group Membership
Taylor, J. Eric T.; Witt, Jessica K.; Grimaldi, Phillip J. – Cognition, 2012
Observed actions are covertly and involuntarily simulated within the observer's motor system. It has been argued that simulation is involved in processing abstract, gestural paintings, as the artist's movements can be simulated by observing static brushstrokes. Though this argument is grounded in theory, empirical research has yet to examine the…
Descriptors: Evidence, Audiences, Artists, Painting (Visual Arts)
Milanese, Nadia; Iani, Cristina; Rubichi, Sandro – Cognition, 2010
We investigated whether performing a task with a co-actor shapes the way a subsequent task is performed. In four experiments participants were administered a Simon task after practicing a spatial compatibility task with an incompatible S-R mapping. In Experiment 1 they performed both tasks alongside another person; in Experiment 2 they performed…
Descriptors: Social Environment, Task Analysis, Spatial Ability, Experiments
Cohen, Adam S.; German, Tamsin C. – Cognition, 2010
In a task where participants' overt task was to track the location of an object across a sequence of events, reaction times to unpredictable probes requiring an inference about a social agent's beliefs about the location of that object were obtained. Reaction times to false belief situations were faster than responses about the (false) contents of…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Beliefs, Child Development, Brain
Parmentier, Fabrice B. R.; Elsley, Jane V.; Ljungberg, Jessica K. – Cognition, 2010
Unexpected events often distract us. In the laboratory, novel auditory stimuli have been shown to capture attention away from a focal visual task and yield specific electrophysiological responses as well as a behavioral cost to performance. Distraction is thought to follow ineluctably from the sound's low probability of occurrence or, put more…
Descriptors: Auditory Stimuli, Probability, Computer Software, Laboratories
Ma, Lili; Xu, Fei – Cognition, 2011
A crucial task in social interaction involves understanding subjective mental states. Here we report two experiments with toddlers exploring whether they can use statistical evidence to infer the subjective nature of preferences. We found that 2-year-olds were likely to interpret another person's nonrandom sampling behavior as a cue for a…
Descriptors: Evidence, Preschool Children, Interpersonal Relationship, Interaction
Campanella, Fabio; Shallice, Tim – Cognition, 2011
While many behavioural studies on refractory phenomena in lexical/semantic access have focused on the mechanisms involved in the oral production of names, comprehension tasks have been almost exclusively used in neuropsychological studies on brain damaged patients. We report the results of two experiments on healthy participants conducted by means…
Descriptors: Semantics, Serial Ordering, Patients, Brain
Sloman, Steven A.; Fernbach, Philip M.; Hagmayer, York – Cognition, 2010
The paper sets out to reveal conditions enabling diagnostic self-deception, people's tendency to deceive themselves about the diagnostic value of their own actions. We characterize different types of self-deception in terms of the distinction between intervention and observation in causal reasoning. One type arises when people intervene but choose…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Intelligence, Educational Policy, Deception
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2