Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 18 |
Descriptor
Experiments | 19 |
Observation | 19 |
Cognitive Processes | 11 |
Models | 5 |
Memory | 4 |
Task Analysis | 4 |
Auditory Stimuli | 3 |
Children | 3 |
Comparative Analysis | 3 |
Evaluation Methods | 3 |
Evidence | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognition | 19 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 19 |
Reports - Research | 15 |
Reports - Evaluative | 4 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 3 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Namibia | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Li, Xiaoou; Mou, Weimin; McNamara, Timothy P. – Cognition, 2012
Four experiments tested whether there are enduring spatial representations of objects' locations in memory. Previous studies have shown that under certain conditions the internal consistency of pointing to objects using memory is disrupted by disorientation. This disorientation effect has been attributed to an absence of or to imprecise enduring…
Descriptors: Memory, Spatial Ability, Experiments, Cognitive Processes
Oeberst, Aileen; Blank, Hartmut – Cognition, 2012
Presenting inconsistent postevent information about a witnessed incident typically decreases the accuracy of memory reports concerning that event (the "misinformation effect"). Surprisingly, the "reversibility" of the effect (after an initial occurrence) has remained largely unexplored. Based on a "memory conversion" theoretical framework and…
Descriptors: Recall (Psychology), Memory, Models, Experiments
Hilbig, Benjamin E. – Cognition, 2012
Extending the well-established negativity bias in human cognition to truth judgments, it was recently shown that negatively framed statistical statements are more likely to be considered true than formally equivalent statements framed positively. However, the underlying processes responsible for this effect are insufficiently understood.…
Descriptors: Response Style (Tests), Value Judgment, Probability, Models
Mattler, Uwe; Palmer, Simon – Cognition, 2012
Unconscious visual stimuli can be processed by human observers and modulate their behavior. This has been shown for masked prime stimuli that influence motor responses to subsequent target stimuli. Beyond this, masked stimuli can also affect participants' behavior when they are free to choose one of two response alternatives. This finding…
Descriptors: Priming, Visual Stimuli, Reaction Time, Cognitive Processes
Bonawitz, Elizabeth; Shafto, Patrick; Gweon, Hyowon; Goodman, Noah D.; Spelke, Elizabeth; Schulz, Laura – Cognition, 2011
Motivated by computational analyses, we look at how teaching affects exploration and discovery. In Experiment 1, we investigated children's exploratory play after an adult pedagogically demonstrated a function of a toy, after an interrupted pedagogical demonstration, after a naive adult demonstrated the function, and at baseline. Preschoolers in…
Descriptors: Evidence, Direct Instruction, Preschool Children, Teaching Methods
Weiss, Carmen; Herwig, Arvid; Schutz-Bosbach, Simone – Cognition, 2011
The immediate experience of self-agency, that is, the experience of generating and controlling our actions, is thought to be a key aspect of selfhood. It has been suggested that this experience is intimately linked to internal motor signals associated with the ongoing actions. These signals should lead to an attenuation of the sensory consequences…
Descriptors: Females, Individual Development, Self Management, Classification
Taylor, J. Eric T.; Witt, Jessica K.; Grimaldi, Phillip J. – Cognition, 2012
Observed actions are covertly and involuntarily simulated within the observer's motor system. It has been argued that simulation is involved in processing abstract, gestural paintings, as the artist's movements can be simulated by observing static brushstrokes. Though this argument is grounded in theory, empirical research has yet to examine the…
Descriptors: Evidence, Audiences, Artists, Painting (Visual Arts)
Lawson, Rebecca – Cognition, 2010
People cannot veridically perceive reflections of objects as projections on the surface of mirrors. People tried to locate an object's projection on a flat mirror. The observer stood at the opposite end of a long mirror to the experimenter. They were told to remember the location of the projection of the experimenter's face. The experimenter then…
Descriptors: Schemata (Cognition), Evaluation Methods, Feedback (Response), Experiments
Beck, Sarah R.; Apperly, Ian A.; Chappell, Jackie; Guthrie, Carlie; Cutting, Nicola – Cognition, 2011
Tool making evidences intelligent, flexible thinking. In Experiment 1, we confirmed that 4- to 7-year-olds chose a hook tool to retrieve a bucket from a tube. In Experiment 2, 3- to 5-year-olds consistently failed to innovate a simple hook tool. Eight-year-olds performed at mature levels. In contrast, making a tool following demonstration was easy…
Descriptors: Experiments, Children, Thinking Skills, Age Differences
Di Luca, Samuel; Lefevre, Nathalie; Pesenti, Mauro – Cognition, 2010
Fingers can be used to express numerical magnitudes, and cultural habits about the fixed order in which fingers are raised determine which configurations become canonical and which non-canonical. Although both types of configuration carry magnitude information, it has been shown that the canonical ones are recognized faster and directly linked to…
Descriptors: Semantics, Number Concepts, Experiments, Cognitive Processes
Pilz, Karin S.; Vuong, Quoc C.; Bulthoff, Heinrich H.; Thornton, Ian M. – Cognition, 2011
A highly familiar type of movement occurs whenever a person walks towards you. In the present study, we investigated whether this type of motion has an effect on face processing. We took a range of different 3D head models and placed them on a single, identical 3D body model. The resulting figures were animated to approach the observer. In a first…
Descriptors: Motion, Visual Perception, Observation, Human Body
Frank, Michael C.; Tenenbaum, Joshua B. – Cognition, 2011
Children learning the inflections of their native language show the ability to generalize beyond the perceptual particulars of the examples they are exposed to. The phenomenon of "rule learning"--quick learning of abstract regularities from exposure to a limited set of stimuli--has become an important model system for understanding generalization…
Descriptors: Native Language, Observation, Models, Language Acquisition
Kalish, Charles W. – Cognition, 2010
Two experiments explored children's and adults' use of examples to make conditional predictions. In Experiment 1 adults (N = 20) but not 4-year-olds (N = 21) or 8-year-olds (N =1 8) distinguished predictable from unpredictable features when features were partially correlated (e.g., necessary but not sufficient). Children did make reliable…
Descriptors: Prediction, Memory, Correlation, Comparative Analysis
Buchsbaum, Daphna; Gopnik, Alison; Griffiths, Thomas L.; Shafto, Patrick – Cognition, 2011
Children are ubiquitous imitators, but how do they decide which actions to imitate? One possibility is that children rationally combine multiple sources of information about which actions are necessary to cause a particular outcome. For instance, children might learn from contingencies between action sequences and outcomes across repeated…
Descriptors: Evidence, Models, Imitation, Preschool Children
Parmentier, Fabrice B. R.; Elsley, Jane V.; Ljungberg, Jessica K. – Cognition, 2010
Unexpected events often distract us. In the laboratory, novel auditory stimuli have been shown to capture attention away from a focal visual task and yield specific electrophysiological responses as well as a behavioral cost to performance. Distraction is thought to follow ineluctably from the sound's low probability of occurrence or, put more…
Descriptors: Auditory Stimuli, Probability, Computer Software, Laboratories
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2