Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 30 |
Descriptor
Models | 31 |
Experiments | 30 |
Cognitive Processes | 8 |
Prediction | 7 |
Visual Stimuli | 6 |
Observation | 5 |
Task Analysis | 5 |
Evaluation Methods | 4 |
Inferences | 4 |
Language Processing | 4 |
Probability | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognition | 31 |
Author
Tenenbaum, Joshua B. | 3 |
Frank, Michael C. | 2 |
Griffiths, Thomas L. | 2 |
Johnson-Laird, P. N. | 2 |
Schiller, Niels O. | 2 |
Aarts, Henk | 1 |
Acres, Kadia | 1 |
Afonso, Olivia | 1 |
Aviles, Alberto | 1 |
Baker, Chris L. | 1 |
Bandi-Rao, Shoba | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 31 |
Reports - Research | 21 |
Reports - Evaluative | 9 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Yildirim, Ilker; Jacobs, Robert A. – Cognition, 2013
We study people's abilities to transfer object category knowledge across visual and haptic domains. If a person learns to categorize objects based on inputs from one sensory modality, can the person categorize these same objects when the objects are perceived through another modality? Can the person categorize novel objects from the same…
Descriptors: Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Stimuli, Infants, Visual Stimuli
Solman, Grayden J. F.; Cheyne, J. Allan; Smilek, Daniel – Cognition, 2012
We present results from five search experiments using a novel "unpacking" paradigm in which participants use a mouse to sort through random heaps of distractors to locate the target. We report that during this task participants often fail to recognize the target despite moving it, and despite having looked at the item. Additionally, the missed…
Descriptors: Evidence, Experiments, Models, Computer Peripherals
Zultan, Ro'i; Gerstenberg, Tobias; Lagnado, David A. – Cognition, 2012
Attributions of responsibility play a critical role in many group interactions. This paper explores the role of causal and counterfactual reasoning in blame attributions in groups. We develop a general framework that builds on the notion of pivotality: an agent is pivotal if she could have changed the group outcome by acting differently. In three…
Descriptors: Cooperative Learning, Interaction, Experiments, Task Analysis
Taylor, Kirsten I.; Devereux, Barry J.; Acres, Kadia; Randall, Billi; Tyler, Lorraine K. – Cognition, 2012
Conceptual representations are at the heart of our mental lives, involved in every aspect of cognitive functioning. Despite their centrality, a long-standing debate persists as to how the meanings of concepts are represented and processed. Many accounts agree that the meanings of concrete concepts are represented by their individual features, but…
Descriptors: Distinctive Features (Language), Identification, Experiments, Models
Juhos, Csongor; Quelhas, Ana Cristina; Johnson-Laird, P. N. – Cognition, 2012
The mental model theory postulates that the meanings of assertions, and knowledge about their context can "modulate" the logical meaning of sentential connectives, such as "if" and "or". One known effect of modulation is to block the representation of possibilities to which a proposition refers. But, modulation should also add relational…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Inferences, Models, Theories
Lai, Jun; Poletiek, Fenna H. – Cognition, 2011
A theoretical debate in artificial grammar learning (AGL) regards the learnability of hierarchical structures. Recent studies using an A[superscript n]B[superscript n] grammar draw conflicting conclusions ([Bahlmann and Friederici, 2006] and [De Vries et al., 2008]). We argue that 2 conditions crucially affect learning A[superscript…
Descriptors: Grammar, Experiments, Models, Cognitive Psychology
Oeberst, Aileen; Blank, Hartmut – Cognition, 2012
Presenting inconsistent postevent information about a witnessed incident typically decreases the accuracy of memory reports concerning that event (the "misinformation effect"). Surprisingly, the "reversibility" of the effect (after an initial occurrence) has remained largely unexplored. Based on a "memory conversion" theoretical framework and…
Descriptors: Recall (Psychology), Memory, Models, Experiments
Jepma, Marieke; Wagenmakers, Eric-Jan; Nieuwenhuis, Sander – Cognition, 2012
People are able to use temporal cues to anticipate the timing of an event, enabling them to process that event more efficiently. We conducted two experiments, using the fixed-foreperiod paradigm (Experiment 1) and the temporal-cueing paradigm (Experiment 2), to assess which components of information processing are speeded when subjects use such…
Descriptors: Expectation, Cues, Reaction Time, Models
Hilbig, Benjamin E. – Cognition, 2012
Extending the well-established negativity bias in human cognition to truth judgments, it was recently shown that negatively framed statistical statements are more likely to be considered true than formally equivalent statements framed positively. However, the underlying processes responsible for this effect are insufficiently understood.…
Descriptors: Response Style (Tests), Value Judgment, Probability, Models
Bogartz, Richard S.; Staub, Adrian – Cognition, 2012
In three experimental tasks Stephen and Mirman (2010) measured gaze steps, the distance in pixels between gaze positions on successive samples from an eyetracker. They argued that the distribution of gaze steps is best fit by the lognormal distribution, and based on this analysis they concluded that interactive cognitive processes underlie eye…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Cognitive Processes, Experiments, Task Analysis
Mattler, Uwe; Palmer, Simon – Cognition, 2012
Unconscious visual stimuli can be processed by human observers and modulate their behavior. This has been shown for masked prime stimuli that influence motor responses to subsequent target stimuli. Beyond this, masked stimuli can also affect participants' behavior when they are free to choose one of two response alternatives. This finding…
Descriptors: Priming, Visual Stimuli, Reaction Time, Cognitive Processes
Dawson, Colin; Gerken, LouAnn – Cognition, 2011
While many constraints on learning must be relatively experience-independent, past experience provides a rich source of guidance for subsequent learning. Discovering structure in some domain can inform a learner's future hypotheses about that domain. If a general property accounts for particular sub-patterns, a rational learner should not…
Descriptors: Evidence, Intervals, Logical Thinking, Auditory Stimuli
Hsu, Anne S.; Chater, Nick; Vitanyi, Paul M. B. – Cognition, 2011
There is much debate over the degree to which language learning is governed by innate language-specific biases, or acquired through cognition-general principles. Here we examine the probabilistic language acquisition hypothesis on three levels: We outline a novel theoretical result showing that it is possible to learn the exact "generative model"…
Descriptors: Linguistics, Prediction, Natural Language Processing, Language Acquisition
Bijleveld, Erik; Custers, Ruud; Aarts, Henk – Cognition, 2010
While both conscious and unconscious reward cues enhance effort to work on a task, previous research also suggests that conscious rewards may additionally affect speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Based on this idea, two experiments explored whether reward cues that are presented above (supraliminal) or below (subliminal) the threshold of conscious…
Descriptors: Cues, Rewards, Hyperactivity, Experiments
Verdonschot, Rinus G.; Middelburg, Renee; Lensink, Saskia E.; Schiller, Niels O. – Cognition, 2012
In a long-lag morphological priming experiment, Dutch (L1)-English (L2) bilinguals were asked to name pictures and read aloud words. A design using non-switch blocks, consisting solely of Dutch stimuli, and switch-blocks, consisting of Dutch primes and targets with intervening English trials, was administered. Target picture naming was facilitated…
Descriptors: Priming, Inhibition, Cognitive Processes, Indo European Languages