NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 63 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kelly, Jonathan W.; Sjolund, Lori A.; Sturz, Bradley R. – Cognition, 2013
Spatial memories are often organized around reference frames, and environmental shape provides a salient cue to reference frame selection. To date, however, the environmental cues responsible for influencing reference frame selection remain relatively unknown. To connect research on reference frame selection with that on orientation via…
Descriptors: Memory, Spatial Ability, Geometric Concepts, Cues
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Brunye, Tad T.; Gagnon, Stephanie A.; Paczynski, Martin; Shenhav, Amitai; Mahoney, Caroline R.; Taylor, Holly A. – Cognition, 2013
Several studies have demonstrated that affective states influence the number of associations formed between remotely related concepts. Someone in a neutral or negative affective state might draw the association between "cold" and "hot", whereas someone in a positive affective state might spontaneously form the more distant association between…
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Semantics, Psychological Patterns, Correlation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Evans, Theodore A.; Beran, Michael J. – Cognition, 2012
Prospective memory (PM) involves forming intentions, retaining those intentions, and later executing those intended responses at the appropriate time. Few studies have investigated this capacity in animals. Monkeys performed a computerized task that assessed their ability to remember to make a particular response if they observed a PM cue embedded…
Descriptors: Memory, Stimuli, Intention, Investigations
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
McGuire, Joseph T.; Kable, Joseph W. – Cognition, 2012
A central question in intertemporal decision making is why people reverse their own past choices. Someone who initially prefers a long-run outcome might fail to maintain that preference for long enough to see the outcome realized. Such behavior is usually understood as reflecting preference instability or self-control failure. However, if a…
Descriptors: Cues, Persistence, Decision Making, Rewards
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Capizzi, Mariagrazia; Sanabria, Daniel; Correa, Angel – Cognition, 2012
The aim of the present study was to investigate the controlled versus the automatic nature of temporal preparation. If temporal preparation involves controlled rather than automatic processing, it should be reduced by the addition of a concurrent demanding task. This hypothesis was tested by comparing participants' performance in a temporal…
Descriptors: Evidence, Cues, Short Term Memory, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Palmer, Terry D.; Ramsey, Ashley K. – Cognition, 2012
The function of consciousness was explored in two contexts of audio-visual speech, cross-modal visual attention guidance and McGurk cross-modal integration. Experiments 1, 2, and 3 utilized a novel cueing paradigm in which two different flash suppressed lip-streams cooccured with speech sounds matching one of these streams. A visual target was…
Descriptors: Attention, Probability, Cues, Lipreading
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cassia, Viola Macchi; Picozzi, Marta; Girelli, Luisa; de Hevia, Maria Dolores – Cognition, 2012
While infants' ability to discriminate quantities has been extensively studied, showing that this competence is present even in neonates, the ability to compute ordinal relations between magnitudes has received much less attention. Here we show that the ability to represent ordinal information embedded in size-based sequences is apparent at 4…
Descriptors: Evidence, Cues, Neonates, Habituation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Green, Jessica J.; Woldorff, Marty G. – Cognition, 2012
The observation of cueing effects (faster responses for cued than uncued targets) rapidly following centrally-presented arrows has led to the suggestion that arrows trigger rapid automatic shifts of spatial attention. However, these effects have primarily been observed during easy target-detection tasks when both cue and target remain on the…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Intervals, Conflict, Attention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Goodhew, Stephanie C.; Visser, Troy A. W.; Lipp, Ottmar V.; Dux, Paul E. – Cognition, 2011
Decades of research on visual perception has uncovered many phenomena, such as binocular rivalry, backward masking, and the attentional blink, that reflect "failures of consciousness". Although stimuli do not reach awareness in these paradigms, there is evidence that they nevertheless undergo semantic processing. Object substitution masking (OSM),…
Descriptors: Semantics, Visual Perception, Cognitive Processes, Cues
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Meyerhoff, Hauke S.; Huff, Markus; Papenmeier, Frank; Jahn, Georg; Schwan, Stephan – Cognition, 2011
Dynamic tasks often require fast adaptations to new viewpoints. It has been shown that automatic spatial updating is triggered by proprioceptive motion cues. Here, we demonstrate that purely visual cues are sufficient to trigger automatic updating. In five experiments, we examined spatial updating in a dynamic attention task in which participants…
Descriptors: Cues, Task Analysis, Spatial Ability, Motion
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Van Opstal, Filip; de Lange, Floris P.; Dehaene, Stanislas – Cognition, 2011
In this study, we investigate whether multiple digits can be processed at a semantic level without awareness, either serially or in parallel. In two experiments, we presented participants with two successive sets of four simultaneous Arabic digits. The first set was masked and served as a subliminal prime for the second, visible target set.…
Descriptors: Evidence, Stimuli, Semantics, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
de Hevia, Maria Dolores – Cognition, 2011
Past research showing a bias towards the larger non-symbolic number by adults and children in line bisection tasks (de Hevia & Spelke, 2009) has been challenged by Gebuis and Gevers, suggesting that area subtended by the stimulus and not number is responsible for the biases. I review evidence supporting the idea that although sensitivity to number…
Descriptors: Evidence, Cues, Form Classes (Languages), Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Caruso, Eugene M.; Waytz, Adam; Epley, Nicholas – Cognition, 2010
People can appear inconsistent in their intuitions about sequences of repeated events. Sometimes people believe such sequences will continue (the "hot hand"), and sometimes people believe they will reverse (the "gambler's fallacy"). These contradictory intuitions can be partly explained by considering the perceived intentionality of the agent…
Descriptors: Prediction, Intuition, Beliefs, Intention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gebuis, Titia; Gevers, Wim – Cognition, 2011
de Hevia and Spelke (de Hevia and Spelke (2009). Spontaneous mapping of number and space in adults and young children, "Cognition, 110", 198-207) investigated the mapping of number onto space. To this end, they introduced a non-symbolic flanker task. Here subjects have to bisect a line that is flanked by a 2-dot and a 9-dot array. Similar to the…
Descriptors: Cues, Cognitive Processes, Investigations, Cognitive Mapping
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Weinbach, Noam; Henik, Avishai – Cognition, 2011
Researchers have suggested that the attention system is composed of several networks that have different functions. One of these networks is responsible for achieving and maintaining an alert state (alerting system), and another for selection and conflict resolution (executive control). There is growing interest in how these attentional networks…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Conflict, Conflict Resolution, Cognitive Processes
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5