Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Source
Child Development | 28 |
Author
Harlaar, Nicole | 2 |
Plomin, Robert | 2 |
Anders, Jake | 1 |
Ariel, Raya | 1 |
Bailey, Drew H. | 1 |
Bates, Timothy C. | 1 |
Bellinger, David | 1 |
Black, F. William | 1 |
Blair, Clancy | 1 |
Blank, Marion | 1 |
Bogin, Barry | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 20 |
Reports - Research | 17 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 4 |
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 2 |
Location
Australia | 2 |
Guatemala | 1 |
Netherlands | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Verwimp, Cara; Snellings, Patrick; Wiers, Reinout W.; Tijms, Jurgen – Child Development, 2023
This study examined how top-down control influenced letter-speech sound (L-SS) learning, the initial phase of learning to read. In 2020, 107 Dutch children (53 boys, M[subscript age] = 106.845 months) learned eight L-SS correspondences, either preceded by goal-directed or implicit instructions. Symbol knowledge and artificial word-reading ability…
Descriptors: Phoneme Grapheme Correspondence, Speech Communication, Language Acquisition, Reading Processes
Parker, Philip D.; Sanders, Taren; Anders, Jake; Parker, Rhiannon B.; Duineveld, Jasper J. – Child Development, 2021
In a representative longitudinal sample of 2,602 Australian children (52% boys; 2% Indigenous; 13% language other than English background; 22% of Mothers born overseas; and 65% Urban) and their mothers (first surveyed in 2003), this article examined if maternal judgments of numeracy and reading ability varied by child demographics and influenced…
Descriptors: Mothers, Parent Attitudes, Numeracy, Reading Ability
Susperreguy, Maria Ines; Davis-Kean, Pamela E.; Duckworth, Kathryn; Chen, Meichu – Child Development, 2018
This study examines whether self-concept of ability in math and reading predicts later math and reading attainment across different levels of achievement. Data from three large-scale longitudinal data sets, the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development--Study of Early Child Care and…
Descriptors: Self Concept, Prediction, Academic Achievement, Role
Bailey, Drew H.; Littlefield, Andrew K. – Child Development, 2017
This study reanalyzes data presented by Ritchie, Bates, and Plomin (2015) who used a cross-lagged monozygotic twin differences design to test whether reading ability caused changes in intelligence. The authors used data from a sample of 1,890 monozygotic twin pairs tested on reading ability and intelligence at five occasions between the ages of 7…
Descriptors: Correlation, Child Development, Intelligence, Developmental Stages
Ritchie, Stuart J.; Bates, Timothy C.; Plomin, Robert – Child Development, 2015
Evidence from twin studies points to substantial environmental influences on intelligence, but the specifics of this influence are unclear. This study examined one developmental process that potentially causes intelligence differences: learning to read. In 1,890 twin pairs tested at 7, 9, 10, 12, and 16 years, a cross-lagged…
Descriptors: Intelligence, Twins, Environmental Influences, Child Development
Harlaar, Nicole; Deater-Deckard, Kirby; Thompson, Lee A.; DeThorne, Laura S.; Petrill, Stephen A. – Child Development, 2011
This study used a cross-lagged twin design to examine reading achievement and independent reading from 10 to 11 years (n = 436 twin pairs). Reading achievement at age 10 significantly predicted independent reading at age 11. The alternative path, from independent reading at age 10 to reading achievement at age 11, was not significant. Individual…
Descriptors: Twins, Independent Reading, Reading Achievement, Genetics
Dickinson, David K.; Porche, Michelle V. – Child Development, 2011
Indirect effects of preschool classroom indexes of teacher talk were tested on fourth-grade outcomes for 57 students from low-income families in a longitudinal study of classroom and home influences on reading. Detailed observations and audiotaped teacher and child language data were coded to measure content and quantity of verbal interactions in…
Descriptors: Reading Comprehension, Play, Low Income, Child Language
Blair, Clancy; Razza, Rachel Peters – Child Development, 2007
This study examined the role of self-regulation in emerging academic ability in one hundred and forty-one 3- to 5-year-old children from low-income homes. Measures of effortful control, false belief understanding, and the inhibitory control and attention-shifting aspects of executive function in preschool were related to measures of math and…
Descriptors: Kindergarten, Academic Ability, Reading Ability, Inhibition

Kershner, John R. – Child Development, 1977
Disabled readers, good readers, and gifted children were compared on their recognition of words presented tachistoscopically under stimultaneous, bilateral viewing conditions. Poor readers showed inferior right-field performance compared with gifted and good readers, but when IQ was covaried, they showed higher left-field scores and lower visual…
Descriptors: Cerebral Dominance, Elementary Education, Reading Ability, Recognition
Harlaar, Nicole; Dale, Philip S.; Plomin, Robert – Child Development, 2007
Little is known about the underlying causes and developmental patterns of stability and change in early reading abilities. In a longitudinal study of twins (n=4,291 pairs), individual differences in reading achievement assessed by teachers using U.K. National Curriculum (NC) criteria showed substantial heritabilities at ages 7, 9, and 10 years…
Descriptors: Early Reading, Environmental Influences, Reading Ability, Reading Achievement

Keenan, Janice M.; Brown, Polly – Child Development, 1984
Examines the differences between beginning and skilled readers in the units used to represent the meaning of text. Compares reading times and recall of 50 third- and fifth-graders. Stimulus sentences all had the same number of words but varied in the number of underlying propositions. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Children, Reading Ability, Reading Achievement, Reading Rate

Kagan, Jerome; And Others – Child Development, 1978
A follow-up investigation of 68 children 10 years of age who had been assessed originally at ages 4, 8, 13, and 27 months, did not reveal strong relations between infant variables (such as attentiveness, vocal excitability, irritability, or activity) and reflection-impulsivity, intelligence quotient, or reading ability at age 10. (JMB)
Descriptors: Conceptual Tempo, Elementary School Students, Followup Studies, Infant Behavior

Waller, T. Gary – Child Development, 1976
A comparison was made between the recognition memory of 18 fifth-grade good readers and 18 fifth-grade poor readers, all males. (BRT)
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students, Grade 5

Guttentag, Robert E.; Haith, Marshall M. – Child Development, 1978
Early and late first-grade children, third-grade poor and good readers, and adults named pictures under several interference conditions: with embedded intracategory or extracategory words, pronounceable or nonpronounceable letter strings, and visual noise. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Decoding (Reading)

Cummings, E. Mark; Faw, Terry T. – Child Development, 1976
Matched groups of normal and retarded readers were required to perform same/difference judgments in which the interval between standard and comparison stimuli was either 0, 1, or 6 seconds. (BRT)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Comparative Analysis, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2