Publication Date
In 2025 | 5 |
Since 2024 | 14 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 63 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 130 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 335 |
Descriptor
Source
Child Development | 746 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 729 |
Reports - Research | 666 |
Reports - Evaluative | 46 |
Information Analyses | 7 |
Reports - Descriptive | 6 |
Opinion Papers | 4 |
Numerical/Quantitative Data | 1 |
Reports - General | 1 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 87 |
Elementary Education | 36 |
Kindergarten | 21 |
Preschool Education | 14 |
Primary Education | 14 |
Grade 1 | 7 |
Grade 2 | 4 |
Adult Education | 3 |
Grade 5 | 3 |
Grade 3 | 2 |
Higher Education | 2 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Researchers | 80 |
Parents | 2 |
Practitioners | 2 |
Location
Canada | 13 |
United States | 8 |
United Kingdom (England) | 7 |
China | 6 |
Australia | 5 |
Germany | 5 |
Romania | 5 |
United Kingdom | 4 |
Brazil | 3 |
France | 3 |
Japan (Tokyo) | 3 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Snowling, Margaret J.; Nash, Hannah M.; Gooch, Debbie C.; Hayiou-Thomas, Marianna E.; Hulme, Charles – Child Development, 2019
We followed children at family risk of dyslexia and children with preschool language difficulties from age 3½, comparing them with controls (N = 234). At age 8, children were classified as having dyslexia or Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) and compared at earlier time points with controls. Children with dyslexia have specific difficulties…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Young Children, Language Impairments, Dyslexia
Tomblin, J. Bruce; Oleson, Jake; Ambrose, Sophie E.; Walker, Elizabeth A.; Moeller, Mary P. – Child Development, 2020
This study contrasted the early literacy outcomes of children who are hard of hearing (CHH) with children with normal hearing (CNH). At age 5, prereading skills of oral language, phonological processing, and print knowledge were examined in CHH (N = 180) and CNH (N = 80). The CHH had poorer oral language and phonological processing abilities than…
Descriptors: Emergent Literacy, Grade 2, Outcomes of Education, Students with Disabilities
Hughes, Claire; Devine, Rory T. – Child Development, 2019
Despite rapidly growing research on parental influences on children's executive function (EF), the uniqueness and specificity of parental predictors and links between adult EF and parenting remain unexamined. This 13-month longitudinal study of 117 parent-child dyads (60 boys; M[subscript age] at Time 1 = 3.94 years, SD = 0.53) included detailed…
Descriptors: Parent Influence, Parent Child Relationship, Executive Function, Predictor Variables
Miller, Jonas G.; Nuselovici, Jacob N.; Hastings, Paul D. – Child Development, 2016
How does empathic physiology unfold as a dynamic process, and which aspect of empathy predicts children's kindness? In response to empathy induction videos, 4- to 6-year-old children (N = 180) showed an average pattern of dynamic respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) change characterized by early RSA suppression, followed by RSA recovery, and modest…
Descriptors: Altruism, Empathy, Psychological Patterns, Prosocial Behavior
Hepach, Robert; Vaish, Amrisha; Grossmann, Tobias; Tomasello, Michael – Child Development, 2016
Children's instrumental helping has sometimes been interpreted as a desire to complete action sequences or to restore the physical order of things. Two-year-old children (n = 51) selectively retrieved for an adult the object he needed rather than one he did not (but which equally served to restore the previous order of things), and those with…
Descriptors: Helping Relationship, Toddlers, Young Children, Arousal Patterns
Botting, Nicola; Jones, Anna; Marshall, Chloe; Denmark, Tanya; Atkinson, Joanna; Morgan, Gary – Child Development, 2017
Studies have suggested that language and executive function (EF) are strongly associated. Indeed, the two are difficult to separate, and it is particularly difficult to determine whether one skill is more dependent on the other. Deafness provides a unique opportunity to disentangle these skills because in this case, language difficulties have a…
Descriptors: Executive Function, Language Impairments, Language Tests, Task Analysis
Tanskanen, Antti O.; Danielsbacka, Mirkka – Child Development, 2018
Associations between grandparental investment and child outcomes were investigated using three waves of a longitudinal British Millennium Cohort Study that included children between the ages of 9 months and 5 years (n = 24,614 person-observations from 13,744 children). Grandparental investment was measured by parent--grandparent contact frequency…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Behavior Problems, Questionnaires, Screening Tests
Gelman, Susan A.; Manczak, Erika M.; Was, Alexandra M.; Noles, Nicholaus S. – Child Development, 2016
An object's mental representation includes not just visible attributes but also its nonvisible history. The present studies tested whether preschoolers seek subtle indicators of an object's history, such as a mark acquired during its handling. Five studies with 169 children 3-5 years of age and 97 college students found that children (like adults)…
Descriptors: Young Children, College Students, Ownership, Comparative Analysis
Howard, Lauren H.; Henderson, Annette M. E.; Carrazza, Cristina; Woodward, Amanda L. – Child Development, 2015
Although children can use social categories to intelligently select informants, children's preference for in-group informants has not been consistently demonstrated across age and context. This research clarifies the extent to which children use social categories to guide learning by presenting participants with a live or video-recorded…
Descriptors: Infants, Young Children, Imitation, Group Membership
Chen, Eva E.; Corriveau, Kathleen H.; Lai, Veronica K.W.; Poon, Sze Long; Gaither, Sarah E. – Child Development, 2018
The impact of social group information on the learning and socializing preferences of Hong Kong Chinese children were examined. Specifically, the degree to which variability in racial out-group exposure affects children's use of race to make decisions about unfamiliar individuals (Chinese, White, Southeast Asian) was investigated. Participants…
Descriptors: Learning Processes, Socialization, Racial Identification, Racial Differences
Sperry, Douglas E.; Sperry, Linda L.; Miller, Peggy J. – Child Development, 2019
Amid growing controversy about the oft-cited "30-million-word gap," this investigation uses language data from five American communities across the socioeconomic spectrum to test, for the first time, Hart and Risley's (1995) claim that poor children hear 30 million fewer words than their middle-class counterparts during the early years…
Descriptors: Socioeconomic Status, Vocabulary Development, Infants, Toddlers
Elenbaas, Laura – Child Development, 2019
This study examined young children's contact with individuals of high-wealth and low-wealth backgrounds and their behavior toward peers of these backgrounds in a resource distribution task. The sample included 72 ethnically diverse higher income children (M[subscript age] = 6.68 years, SD = 0.98 years). Contact with individuals of low-wealth…
Descriptors: Advantaged, Family Income, Low Income, Peer Relationship
Neary, Karen R.; Friedman, Ori – Child Development, 2014
This study provides evidence that children give priority to ownership when judging who should use an object. Children (N = 269) and adults (N = 154) considered disputes over objects. In disputes between a character using an object and the owner of the object, children, as young as 3 years and as old as 7 years, sided with the owner, and did so…
Descriptors: Young Children, Ownership, Childhood Attitudes, Child Development
Kahrs, Björn Alexander; Jung, Wendy P.; Lockman, Jeffrey J. – Child Development, 2014
This study examines the development of hammering within an ontogenetic and evolutionary framework using motion-capture technology. Twenty-four right-handed toddlers (19-35 months) wore reflective markers while hammering a peg into a peg-board. The study focuses on the motor characteristics that make tool use uniquely human: wrist involvement,…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Young Children, Motor Development, Handedness
Redshaw, Jonathan; Suddendorf, Thomas; Neldner, Karri; Wilks, Matti; Tomaselli, Keyan; Mushin, Ilana; Nielsen, Mark – Child Development, 2019
This study examined future-oriented behavior in children (3-6 years; N = 193) from three diverse societies--one industrialized Western city and two small, geographically isolated communities. Children had the opportunity to prepare for two alternative versions of an immediate future event over six trials. Some 3-year-olds from all cultures…
Descriptors: Futures (of Society), Toddlers, Young Children, Cultural Differences