Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 5 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 30 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 54 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 168 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 521 |
Age Differences | 158 |
Cognitive Development | 132 |
Children | 109 |
Elementary School Students | 100 |
Child Development | 92 |
Preschool Children | 89 |
Memory | 82 |
Infants | 73 |
Young Children | 70 |
Visual Stimuli | 54 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 521 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 424 |
Reports - Research | 371 |
Reports - Evaluative | 28 |
Opinion Papers | 12 |
Information Analyses | 10 |
Reports - Descriptive | 8 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 29 |
Elementary Education | 24 |
Preschool Education | 16 |
Grade 1 | 6 |
Grade 5 | 6 |
Kindergarten | 6 |
Primary Education | 5 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 3 |
Grade 2 | 3 |
Grade 3 | 3 |
Grade 10 | 2 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Researchers | 47 |
Location
Australia | 4 |
Canada | 3 |
Germany | 3 |
California | 2 |
China | 2 |
South Korea | 2 |
United Kingdom | 2 |
United Kingdom (England) | 2 |
Belgium | 1 |
Cyprus | 1 |
Finland | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating

Sonnenschein, Susan – Child Development, 1982
Three experiments investigated the conditions under which redundant verbal information would facilitate a listener's performance. Kindergarteners, first graders, and fourth graders were asked to select which of several groups of pictures a message (either redundant or constructive) described. Verbal redundancy was found to facilitate only older…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Communication Research, Elementary School Students

Kirsh, Steven J.; Cassidy, Jude – Child Development, 1997
Examined the relationship between infants' attachment quality and attention and memory at 3.5 years. Found that insecure/avoidant and insecure/ambivalent children looked away from mother-child drawings more than secure children. Secure children better recalled stories in which mothers responded sensitively than did insecure/avoidant children, and…
Descriptors: Attachment Behavior, Attention, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes

Sloutsky, Vladimir M.; Napolitano, Amanda C. – Child Development, 2003
Four experiments tested the hypothesis that the importance of linguistic labels for young children's conceptual organization stems from a privileged processing status of auditory input over visual input. Findings indicated that when auditory and visual stimuli were presented separately, 4-year-olds were likely to process both kinds of stimuli,…
Descriptors: Auditory Perception, Auditory Stimuli, Classification, Cognitive Processes

Deak, Gedeon O.; Ray, Shanna D.; Brenneman, Kimberly – Child Development, 2003
Two experiments examined the communicative bases of preschoolers' object appearance-reality (AR) errors. Found that AR performance correlated positively with performance on a control test with the same discourse structure but nondeceptive stimuli, and on a naming test. Overall findings indicated that the discourse structure of AR tests elicits a…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Error Patterns, Language Skills

Uttal, David H. – Child Development, 1996
Preschoolers, elementary school children, and adults reconstructed configurations of objects depicted on maps of empty rooms. Found that with symmetric configurations, most subjects preserved the configuration of objects, but preschoolers placed objects far from correct locations; preschoolers performed worse with asymmetric than symmetric…
Descriptors: Adults, Cognitive Processes, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students

Smetana, Judith G. – Child Development, 1989
Results suggested that preadolescents and adolescents understand but reject or subordinate parents' conventional interpretations of family conflict, and reinterpret them as issues of personal jurisdiction. Parents understand but reject children's claims to personal jurisdiction, and state the issues in conventional terms. (RH)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Age Differences, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Processes

Baillargeon, Renee; DeVos, Julie – Child Development, 1991
Observed the reactions of 3.5-month-old infants looking at a carrot that should have but did not appear in a window after passing behind a screen. The results of this and several similar experiments indicated that 3.5-month-old infants are able to represent and reason about hidden objects. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Concept Formation

Lillard, Angeline S. – Child Development, 1993
Four experiments confirmed the widely accepted hypothesis that, although children as young as two engage in pretend play, even four and five year olds do not understand that pretending requires mental representation. Children appear to misconstrue pretense as its common external manifestations, such as actions, until at least age six. (MDM)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Developmental Stages, Early Childhood Education

Jacobson, Sandra W.; And Others – Child Development, 1993
A total of 403 black, inner-city infants born to women recruited prenatally on basis of their alcohol consumption during pregnancy were assessed on a battery of tests focusing on information processing and complexity of play. Increased prenatal alcohol exposure was associated with longer fixation duration, a result indicative of less efficient…
Descriptors: Black Mothers, Black Youth, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Processes

Keating, Daniel P.; Bobbitt, Bruce L. – Child Development, 1978
Three experiments (simple versus choice reaction time, Posner letter identification, and Sternberg memory scanning) attempted to determine whether reliable individual differences in cognitive processing exist in children and, if so, whether these differences are systematically related to age and ability. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Ability, Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Elementary School Students

Carter, D. Bruce; Levy, Gary D. – Child Development, 1988
Results suggest that gender schematization exerts an important influence in early sex-role development and illustrate the utility of a gender schematic approach to early sex-typing phenomena. (RH)
Descriptors: Activities, Cognitive Processes, Individual Development, Memory

Gibbons, Jane; And Others – Child Development, 1986
Compares the effects of audio and audiovisual presentation on young children's cognitive processing while explicitly controlling the amount and complexity of information. (HOD)
Descriptors: Audiovisual Aids, Auditory Stimuli, Cognitive Processes, Comparative Analysis

Komatsu, Lloyd K.; Galotti, Kathleen M. – Child Development, 1986
Reports on two studies during which 6-, 8-, and 10-year-old children were interviewed about three different types of regularities or rules: social conventions, physical laws, and logical necessities. Shows that older children made more distinctions between social and nonsocial items than did younger children. (HOD)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Concept Formation

Ackerman, Brian P. – Child Development, 1986
Investigates whether 7- and 10-year-old children and adults are sensitive to their own and another listener's failure to understand literal and nonliteral (sarcastic) uses of utterances. (HOD)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Auditory Perception, Cognitive Processes, Developmental Stages

Rose, Susan A.; Wallace, Ina F. – Child Development, 1985
Infant novelty scores correlated significantly with measures of cognitive outcome beginning at 24 months of age and continuing at 34, 40, and 72 months of age. Parental education was strongly correlated with cognitive outcome beginning at about two years of age. (RH)
Descriptors: Academic Achievement, Cognitive Processes, Memory, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)