Publication Date
In 2025 | 5 |
Since 2024 | 14 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 63 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 130 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 335 |
Descriptor
Source
Child Development | 746 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 729 |
Reports - Research | 666 |
Reports - Evaluative | 46 |
Information Analyses | 7 |
Reports - Descriptive | 6 |
Opinion Papers | 4 |
Numerical/Quantitative Data | 1 |
Reports - General | 1 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 87 |
Elementary Education | 36 |
Kindergarten | 21 |
Preschool Education | 14 |
Primary Education | 14 |
Grade 1 | 7 |
Grade 2 | 4 |
Adult Education | 3 |
Grade 5 | 3 |
Grade 3 | 2 |
Higher Education | 2 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Researchers | 80 |
Parents | 2 |
Practitioners | 2 |
Location
Canada | 13 |
United States | 8 |
United Kingdom (England) | 7 |
China | 6 |
Australia | 5 |
Germany | 5 |
Romania | 5 |
United Kingdom | 4 |
Brazil | 3 |
France | 3 |
Japan (Tokyo) | 3 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating

Eisele, Julie; Lust, Barbara – Child Development, 1996
Children and adults made truth-value judgments on matches between pictures and sentences with pronouns and possible antecedents. Results revealed the role of dependence on grammatical structure in pronoun interpretation for all ages; a significant effect of pronoun directionality (position relative to antecedent); and adults' bias related to…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Language Usage, Pragmatics

Chandler, Michael; And Others – Child Development, 1989
Investigated the ability of 56 children of 2-4 years to deceptively lead others into false beliefs. Results show that 2 1/2-year-olds already practice a variety of deceptive strategies that presuppose an operative theory of mind. (RJC)
Descriptors: Beliefs, Child Development, Cognitive Development, Deception

Baldwin, Dare A. – Child Development, 1989
Expectations concerning form and color in object label referencing of 80 children of 2-3 years were examined in 2 studies. Findings show that children as young as 2 expect form similarity to be a better guide than color similarity to the extension of object labels. (RJC)
Descriptors: Classification, Color, Developmental Stages, Learning Processes

Lay, Keng-Ling; And Others – Child Development, 1989
Two studies examined the relationship between maternal responsiveness, child compliance, and mood in 54 mothers and their 4-year-old children. Responsive play increased positive mood but did not affect arousal levels. Children induced into positive moods complied more and with shorter latencies than children induced into negative moods. (RJC)
Descriptors: Compliance (Psychology), Mothers, Parent Child Relationship, Play

Kagan, Jerome; And Others – Child Development, 1989
Investigates the preservation of inhibited and uninhibited behaviors in 100 children of 14, 20, 32, and 48 months. Children who had been extremely inhibited or uninhibited at 14 and 20 months differed significantly at 4 years of age in behavior and cardiac acceleration. (RJC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Heart Rate, Inhibition, Longitudinal Studies

Cortez, Victoria L.; Bugental, Daphne Blunt – Child Development, 1995
Young children watched videotaped fairy tales that involved child or adult control over frightening events. Subsequently, they watched a videotape of a child having a medical exam. Children who had watched the child control fairy tales showed an enhancement, whereas children who had watched the adult control fairy tales showed a deficit, in…
Descriptors: Attention, Cognitive Processes, Fear, Locus of Control

Mitchell, P.; Robinson, E. J. – Child Development, 1994
Three experiments tested four- to seven-year olds' ability to understand and reconcile message-desire discrepant stories. The findings suggest that young children can refrain from a performative response and, as a consequence, attend to literal meaning under some conditions when evaluating utterances. (MDM)
Descriptors: Beliefs, Childhood Attitudes, Cognitive Development, Evaluation

Craton, Lincoln G.; And Others – Child Development, 1990
In a study of four-, six-, and eight-year olds, communication about the left-right dimension proved to be particularly difficult for four-year olds. Frames of reference that children incorporated into their directions changed with age and differed for directions about front-back and left-right dimensions. (RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Communication (Thought Transfer), Performance Factors, Spatial Ability

Goodman, Gail S.; Aman, Christine – Child Development, 1990
Three- and five-year olds were tested for recall with anatomically detailed dolls and regular dolls. Subjects were tested with and without visual cues. Anatomically detailed dolls did not foster false reports of abuse. Findings have implications for children's testimony in child abuse cases. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Abuse, Cues, Recall (Psychology)

Friedman, William J. – Child Development, 1991
In this study of the distinction between temporal distance and location, children were asked to judge the relative recency and time of target events that occurred one and seven weeks before testing. All judged recency and localized time of day correctly. Six- and eight- but not four-year olds localized longer time scales. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cues, Individual Development, Memory

Ward, Thomas B. – Child Development, 1990
Addresses Nelson's commentary on Ward, Vela, and Hass' study of children's category learning (both of which are in this issue). Discusses the issue of whether a holistic processing view provides a better account of children's learning than does an analytical view. (PCB)
Descriptors: Child Development, Classification, Concept Formation, Holistic Approach

Moore, Chris; And Others – Child Development, 1990
Two studies showed that children of about four years of age had begun to understand that beliefs may be held with differing degrees of certainty. This understanding was tied to the children's understanding of the nature of beliefs and the distinction between appearance and reality. (PCB)
Descriptors: Beliefs, Child Development, Cognitive Development, Foreign Countries

Estes, David – Child Development, 1998
Four-year olds, 6-year olds, and adults were given a computer-game mental rotation task, but with no instructions on mental rotation or other mental activity. Reaction time patterns and verbal reports revealed that 6-year olds were comparable to adults in spontaneous use and subjective awareness of mental rotation. Four-year olds who referred to…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Metacognition

Brooks, Patricia J.; Tomasello, Michael; Dodson, Kelly; Lewis, Lawrence B. – Child Development, 1999
Examined children's tendency to make argument structure overgeneralization errors. Found that 3- to 8-year-olds were more likely to overgeneralize verbs less familiar to them, supporting the hypothesis that verb usage in particular construction types becomes entrenched over time. As children learn transitivity status of particular verbs, they…
Descriptors: Child Language, Familiarity, Generalization, Language Acquisition
DiPietro, Janet A.; Novak, Matthew F. S. X.; Costigan, Kathleen A.; Atella, Lara D.; Reusing, Sarah P. – Child Development, 2006
Concern exists that a constellation of negative maternal emotions during pregnancy generates persistent negative consequences for child development. Maternal reports of anxiety, pregnancy-specific and nonspecific stress, and depressive symptoms were collected during mid-pregnancy and at 6 weeks and 24 months after birth in a sample of healthy…
Descriptors: Anxiety, Depression (Psychology), Pregnancy, Child Development