NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Source
Child Development1526
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Showing 406 to 420 of 1,526 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Arsenio, William F.; Kramer, Rivka – Child Development, 1992
In two experiments, children observed pictures of moral transgressions. Most children expected victimizers to feel positive emotions and victims to feel negative emotions. However, in the second experiment, 4 year olds attributed extremely positive emotions to victimizers, whereas 8 year olds attributed less positive emotions to victimizers. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Emotional Response, Social Cognition, Victimization
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Robinson, J. A.; And Others – Child Development, 1990
Results of three experiments support the conclusion that tasks involving the localization of objects or events from mirror images are not direct indices of self-recognition among children between 14 and 22 months of age. Rather, they indicate the skill of infants in using the mirror as a perceptual tool. (RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Ability, Difficulty Level, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Adolph, Karen E.; And Others – Child Development, 1993
Examined the behavior of 8.5-month-old crawling infants and 14-month-old walking toddlers in ascending and descending sloping walkways. Both groups overestimated their ability to ascend slopes. Toddlers hesitated most before descending 10 and 20 degree slopes, whereas infants hesitated most before descending 30 and 40 degree slopes. (MDM)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Infants, Perception, Psychomotor Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Spetner, Nancy Benson; Olsho, Lynne Werner – Child Development, 1990
Uses a nonsimultaneous pulsation threshold technique to examine the development of frequency resolutions in infants of three to six months. (PCB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Auditory Discrimination, Auditory Stimuli, Child Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Brownell, Celia A.; Carriger, Michael Sean – Child Development, 1990
Children at 12, 18, 24, and 30 months of age were observed in same-age, same-sex dyads while they attempted to solve a simple cooperation problem. Each child was given an elicited imitation task that was used to index decentration. Although no dyad of 12 month olds could cooperate, 24 and 30 month olds could coordinate behavior quickly and…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cooperation, Individual Development, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wainryb, Cecilia; Shaw, Leigh A.; Langley, Marcie; Cottam, Kim; Lewis, Renee – Child Development, 2004
Children's thinking about diversity of belief in 4 realms morality, taste, facts, and ambiguous facts was examined. Ninety-six participants (ages 5, 7, and 9) were interviewed about beliefs different from their own that were endorsed by characters with different status; their judgments of relativism, tolerance, and disagreeing persons were…
Descriptors: Young Children, Beliefs, Concept Formation, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Plumert, Jodie M.; Kearney, Joseph K.; Cremer, James F. – Child Development, 2004
This study examined gap choices and crossing behavior in children and adults using an immersive, interactive bicycling simulator. Ten- and 12-year-olds and adults rode a bicycle mounted on a stationary trainer through a virtual environment consisting of a street with 6 intersections. Participants faced continuous cross traffic traveling at 25mph…
Descriptors: Physical Activities, Age Differences, Preadolescents, Traffic Safety
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Colombo, John; Kannass, Kathleen N.; Jill Shaddy, D.; Kundurthi, Shashi; Maikranz, Julie M.; Anderson, Christa J.; Blaga, Otilia M.; Carlson, Susan E. – Child Development, 2004
Infants were followed longitudinally to document the relationship between docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) levels and the development of attention. Erythrocyte (red-blood cell; RBC) phospholipid DHA (percentage of total fatty acids) was measured from infants and mothers at delivery. Infants were assessed in infant-control habituation at 4, 6, and 8…
Descriptors: Mothers, Cognitive Development, Infants, Habituation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rai, Roshan; Mitchell, Peter – Child Development, 2006
Do young children appreciate the importance of access to premises when judging what another person knows? In Experiment 1, 5-year-olds (N=31) were sensitive to another person's access to premises when predicting that person's ability to point to a target after eliminating alternatives in a set of 3 cartoon characters. Experiment 2 replicated the…
Descriptors: Inferences, Cartoons, Young Children, Access to Information
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Alink, Lenneke R. A.; Mesman, Judi; van Zeijl, Jantien; Stolk, Mirjam N.; Juffer, Femmie; Koot, Hans M.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, Marian J.; van I Jzendoorn, Marinus H. – Child Development, 2006
This study examines the prevalence, stability, and development of physical aggression, as reported by mothers and fathers, in a sample of children initially recruited at 12, 24, and 36 months (N=2,253) and in a subsample followed up 1 year later (n=271) in a cross-sequential design. Physical aggression occurred in 12-month-olds, but significantly…
Descriptors: Young Children, Aggression, Incidence, Child Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Masangkay, Zenaida S.; And Others – Child Development, 1974
Three experiments assessed the ability of 2 to 5-year-old children to infer, under very simple task conditions, what another person sees when viewing something from a position other than the children's own. (ST)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Egocentrism, Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hewitt, Lynn Stewart – Child Development, 1975
Dutch boys, 8 and 12 years old, read brief stories about a harm-doer whose intentions were either good or bad and whose actions resulted in either minor or serious injury to a victim. The older boys' but not the younger boys, differentiated naughtiness on the basis of provocation and intentions. (Author/CS)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students, Moral Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lyons-Ruth, Karlen – Child Development, 1978
Children aged two and one-half to five years gave moral evaluations, attributions of parental affect, and personal liking evaluations of both standard (motive and outcome) moral episodes and simplified (motive only) episodes. (JMB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Moral Development, Perspective Taking, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Wannemacher, Jill T.; Ryan, Mary Lee – Child Development, 1978
Examined the distinction between preschool children's incorrect and opposite interpretations of "less" and investigated the influence of contextual/procedural factors on their comprehension of "less." (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Comprehension, Fundamental Concepts, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Zukier, Henry; Hagen, John William – Child Development, 1978
Sixty children at each of two age levels (8 and 11 years old) performed a serial position recall task either in a control condition or under visual or auditory distraction and were tested for recall of task-relevant and task-irrelevant information. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Attention Control, Elementary School Students, Recall (Psychology)
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  24  |  25  |  26  |  27  |  28  |  29  |  30  |  31  |  32  |  ...  |  102