Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 5 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 30 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 54 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 168 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 521 |
Age Differences | 158 |
Cognitive Development | 132 |
Children | 109 |
Elementary School Students | 100 |
Child Development | 92 |
Preschool Children | 89 |
Memory | 82 |
Infants | 73 |
Young Children | 70 |
Visual Stimuli | 54 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 521 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 424 |
Reports - Research | 371 |
Reports - Evaluative | 28 |
Opinion Papers | 12 |
Information Analyses | 10 |
Reports - Descriptive | 8 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 29 |
Elementary Education | 24 |
Preschool Education | 16 |
Grade 1 | 6 |
Grade 5 | 6 |
Kindergarten | 6 |
Primary Education | 5 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 3 |
Grade 2 | 3 |
Grade 3 | 3 |
Grade 10 | 2 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Researchers | 47 |
Location
Australia | 4 |
Canada | 3 |
Germany | 3 |
California | 2 |
China | 2 |
South Korea | 2 |
United Kingdom | 2 |
United Kingdom (England) | 2 |
Belgium | 1 |
Cyprus | 1 |
Finland | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating

And Others; Cohen, Robert – Child Development, 1979
First and fifth graders estimated all interlocation distances among six objects in one of two environments: a familiar library or a novel arrangement of objects in a room. Findings suggest (1) an increase in the ability to shift among spatial frames of reference with development and (2) a sequence of mastery of spatial knowledge. (RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Elementary School Students, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)

Bushnell, Emily W.; And Others – Child Development, 1995
Examined the ability of 1-year olds to remember the location of nonvisible targets. Found that infants were able to associate a nonvisible target with a direct landmark and to code its distance and direction with respect to themselves or the larger framework. Difficulty of coding with indirect landmarks was associated with cognitive complexity and…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Cues, Infants

Thompson, Douglas R.; Siegler, Robert S. – Child Development, 2000
Two experiments examined development of economic understanding among 5-, 7- and 9-year-olds. Found that most 5-year-olds understood the goal of acquiring desired goods, and most 7- and 9-year-olds also understood the goals of seeking profits, acquiring goods inexpensively, and competing successfully with other sellers. Results suggest that older…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development

Heyman, Gail D.; Dweck, Carol S. – Child Development, 1998
Investigated the relation between 7- and 8-year-olds' interpretations of human behavior and children's beliefs about the stability of human traits. Found that the belief that traits are stable predicted a greater tendency to make trait judgments in the academic and sociomoral domains, and an increased focus on outcomes such as performance and…
Descriptors: Childhood Attitudes, Cognitive Processes, Evaluative Thinking, Personality Traits

Bialystok, Ellen – Child Development, 1999
Investigated in preschoolers whether the bilingual advantage in cognitive control or selective attention could be found in a nonverbal task, the dimensional change card sort, requiring minimal demands for analysis or representation. Found that bilingual children were more advanced than monolinguals in solving problems requiring high levels of…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Cognitive Processes, Comparative Analysis, Language Processing

Jankowski, Jeffery J.; Rose, Susan A.; Feldman, Judith F. – Child Development, 2001
Studied in three experiments the distribution and malleability of visual attention in 5-month-olds while they inspected large geometric designs. Established that infants who were short-lookers had novelty scores above chance, whereas long-lookers demonstrated chance responding. Illuminating different parts of visual display induced long-lookers to…
Descriptors: Attention, Attention Control, Cognitive Processes, Infant Behavior
Luna,Beatriz; Garver,Krista E.; Urban,Trinity A.; Lazar,Nicole A.; Sweeney,John A. – Child Development, 2004
To characterize cognitive maturation through adolescence, processing speed, voluntary response suppression, and spatial working memory were measured in 8- to 30-year-old (N=245) healthy participants using oculomotor tasks. Development progressed with a steep initial improvement in performance followed by stabilization in adolescence. Adult-level…
Descriptors: Memory, Cognitive Processes, Adolescent Development, Adolescents
Bremner, J. Gavin; Johnson, Scott P.; Slater, Alan; Mason, Uschi; Foster, Kirsty; Cheshire, Andrea; Spring, Joanne – Child Development, 2005
When an object moves behind an occluder and re-emerges, 4-month-old infants perceive trajectory continuity only when the occluder is narrow, raising the question of whether time or distance out of sight is the important constraining variable. One hundred and forty 4-month-olds were tested in five experiments aimed to disambiguate time and distance…
Descriptors: Infants, Infant Behavior, Perceptual Development, Visual Perception
Berger, Sarah E.; Adolph, Karen E.; Lobo, Sharon A. – Child Development, 2005
This study examined whether 16-month-old walking infants take the material composition of a handrail into account when assessing its effectiveness as a tool to augment balance. Infants were encouraged to cross from one platform to another via bridges of various widths (10, 20, 40cm) with either a wobbly (foam or latex) or a wooden handrail…
Descriptors: Child Development, Physical Activities, Infant Behavior, Toddlers

Steiner, Gerhard – Child Development, 1974
Piaget's concept of "internalization" is analyzed and related to Bruner's representational theories of cognitive development. Cognitive structures are shown to be real exclusively in an actual representation. (Author/SDH)
Descriptors: Child Development, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Comparative Analysis

Moir, D. John – Child Development, 1974
The development of moral judgment in 11-year-old girls is discussed in terms of the evaluation of role taking ability. (ST)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Elementary School Students, Females

Levin, Joel R.; And Others – Child Development, 1975
Investigated whether there exists a stage of development at which "imagery-inducing motor activity" ceases to be facilitative relative to the effectiveness of alternative kinds of associative-learning strategies. (Author/SDH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Elementary School Students, Imagery, Motor Development

Linn, Marcia C. – Child Development, 1978
Reports two experiments which examine how cognitive style interacts with performance on tasks measuring formal thought. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Style, Elementary School Students, Junior High School Students

Cummings, E. Mark; Faw, Terry T. – Child Development, 1976
Matched groups of normal and retarded readers were required to perform same/difference judgments in which the interval between standard and comparison stimuli was either 0, 1, or 6 seconds. (BRT)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Comparative Analysis, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students

Callanan, Maureen A. – Child Development, 1985
Reports the results of one study in which parents taught their two- to four-year-olds basic and superordinate concepts, and another, in which they taught them subordinate concepts. Parents' teaching styles were analyzed in terms of their usefulness for children who are attempting to learn about principles of hierarchical classification. (AS)
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Processes, Concept Formation, Language