Publication Date
In 2025 | 2 |
Since 2024 | 4 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 20 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 41 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 108 |
Descriptor
Source
Child Development | 409 |
Author
Wellman, Henry M. | 11 |
Gelman, Susan A. | 7 |
Peterson, Candida C. | 7 |
Dweck, Carol S. | 5 |
Gopnik, Alison | 4 |
Koerber, Susanne | 4 |
Osterhaus, Christopher | 4 |
Ruble, Diane N. | 4 |
Abramovitch, Rona | 3 |
Apperly, Ian A. | 3 |
Belsky, Jay | 3 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 338 |
Reports - Research | 250 |
Opinion Papers | 62 |
Information Analyses | 33 |
Reports - Evaluative | 13 |
Reports - Descriptive | 6 |
Reports - General | 1 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 36 |
Location
Israel | 5 |
Germany | 4 |
Canada | 3 |
California | 2 |
China | 2 |
Switzerland | 2 |
United Kingdom | 2 |
United Kingdom (England) | 2 |
United States | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
Austria | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Child Behavior Checklist | 1 |
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 1 |
Social Skills Rating System | 1 |
Strengths and Difficulties… | 1 |
Wechsler Intelligence Scale… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 1 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 2 |
Does not meet standards | 1 |

Ruble, Diane N.; And Others – Child Development, 1976
In 2 studies, age differences in children's self-evaluative responses as a function of success/failure outcome and task ease information were explored. (SB)
Descriptors: Achievement Rating, Age Differences, Attribution Theory, Elementary Education

Langford, Peter E. – Child Development, 1997
Two studies used a modification of the weakly interpretive scoring method of Langford and D'Cruz to examine judicial and legislative reasoning. Findings were in accord with modified versions of Piaget's and Kohlberg's views and contradicted Gibbs' theory. There were three stages of legislative reasoning between 7 and 21 years: heteronomy or…
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Moral Development, Moral Values, Theories

Krascum, Ruth M.; Andrews, Sally – Child Development, 1998
Two experiments examined 4- to 5-year-olds' acquisition of family-resemblance categories for fictitious animals. Results showed that children who performed theory-guided learning were more successful at making feature/category associations than children who performed similarity-guided learning and categorized attributes significantly better than…
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation, Performance Factors

Grych, John H.; Fincham, Frank D.; Jouriles, Ernest N.; McDonald, Renee – Child Development, 2000
Examined processes involved in the relationship of early adolescents' appraisals of interparental conflict with adjustment problems in two samples. Found that perceived threat mediated association between interparental conflict and internalizing problems in both samples; self-blame mediated this association for boys in both samples and girls in…
Descriptors: Childhood Attitudes, Children, Conflict, Emotional Adjustment

McCune, Lorraine – Child Development, 1998
Play has been difficult to define because it is an aspect of many activities rather than of just a specific kind of activity. Classic theorists such as Piaget and Vygotsky emphasized representational play as play in its purist form, but both immediate and ultimate functions of play can be discerned in simple physical activity play. (Author)
Descriptors: Child Development, Definitions, Evolution, Physical Activities

Astington, Janet Wilde – Child Development, 2001
Offers suggestions for future investigations of theory-of-mind development. Maintains that there needs to be: (1) greater focus on the development of understanding of desire and intention; (2) research on the role of language in theory-of-mind development integrating representational-development and social-interaction views; and (3) investigation…
Descriptors: Beliefs, Children, Cognitive Development, Intention

Scholl, Brian J.; Leslie, Alan M. – Child Development, 2001
Maintains that the results of Wellman, Cross, and Watson's meta-analysis on the false belief task are perfectly compatible with "early competence" accounts that posit a specific, innate, and possibly modular basis for theory of mind. Asserts that Wellman and colleagues' arguments against such views stem from mistaken assumptions…
Descriptors: Child Development, Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes

Friedman, William J. – Child Development, 2001
Three experiments examined 3- to 11-year-olds' understanding of entropy, asking whether undifferentiated forces, such as the wind or objects being thrown into the air, could create order or disorder in a set of objects. Found that even 4-year-olds were sensitive to asymmetrical effects of such events. Older children applied this principle more…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation
Updegraff, Kimberly A.; McHale, Susan M.; Whiteman, Shawn D.; Thayer, Shawna M.; Crouter, Ann C. – Child Development, 2006
Drawing on cultural-ecological and person-environment fit perspectives, this study examined links among Mexican-American adolescents' time with peers and parents, parents' cultural orientations, and adolescents' psychosocial adjustment and cultural orientations. Participants were 492 Mexican-American adolescents (Ms=15.7 and 12.8 years for older…
Descriptors: Siblings, Family Relationship, Adolescents, Correlation

Lord, Catherine – Child Development, 1974
An examination of the extent to which adults and children (7 and 11 years old) were able to make discriminations between fixations directed at their eyes and at different positions on their faces. (SDH)
Descriptors: Adults, Elementary School Students, Eye Fixations, Learning Theories

Hook, Jay – Child Development, 1978
Children aged 5, 9, and 13 years did work superior to, equal to, or inferior to another child and allocated reward between them. The three age groups showed qualitatively different allocations consistent with hypothesized age-related stages: (1) no relation of allocation to relative work, (2) preservation of rank from work to reward, and (3)…
Descriptors: Elementary School Students, Logical Thinking, Mathematical Concepts, Research

Abramovitch, Rona; Daly, Eleanor M. – Child Development, 1978
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Attribution Theory, Elementary School Students, Nonverbal Communication

Weisz, John R. – Child Development, 1978
Discusses principles of human development which are durable across changes in time, culture, and cohort. Analyzes the form these principles are likely to take, the limitations and strengths they are likely to display, and the process by which they are likely to be discovered. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Cultural Context, Developmental Psychology, Human Development, Research Methodology

Berndt, Thomas J. – Child Development, 1977
Kindergarten children and adults were shown slides with an accompanying taped soundtrack which portrayed reciprocal and nonreciprocal aggressive and prosocial interactions. Following each episode, subjects' evaluations of the actor and their attributions concerning the cause of his behavior were obtained. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Aggression, Attribution Theory, College Students, Kindergarten Children

Eisenberg, Nancy; And Others – Child Development, 1984
Examines the relation of preschoolers' self-attributions about their prosocial behaviors to frequency of prosocial responding. Attempts to determine if different types of prosocial behaviors are associated with different configurations of moral judgment, self-attributions, and social behaviors. Classroom observations of 44 preschoolers were made…
Descriptors: Attribution Theory, Moral Values, Preschool Children, Preschool Education