Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7 |
Descriptor
Children | 18 |
Perspective Taking | 18 |
Age Differences | 10 |
Social Cognition | 8 |
Cognitive Development | 5 |
Cognitive Processes | 4 |
Adolescents | 3 |
Adults | 3 |
Comparative Analysis | 3 |
Moral Development | 3 |
Childhood Attitudes | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 18 |
Author
Abrams, Dominic | 2 |
Apperly, Ian A. | 1 |
Astington, Janet Wilde | 1 |
Aïte, Ania | 1 |
Baker, Bruce L. | 1 |
Barrett, Justin L. | 1 |
Berthoz, Alain | 1 |
Borst, Grégoire | 1 |
Brody, Gene H. | 1 |
Cutrona, Carolyn E. | 1 |
Dollinger, Stephen J. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 18 |
Reports - Research | 17 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Australia | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Aïte, Ania; Berthoz, Alain; Vidal, Julie; Roëll, Margot; Zaoui, Mohamed; Houdé, Olivier; Borst, Grégoire – Child Development, 2016
To determine whether the growing ability to take a third-person perspective (3PP) is explained in part by the growing ability to inhibit a first-person perspective (1PP), 10-year-old children (n = 49) and 22-year-old adults (n = 52) performed a negative priming adaptation of the own body transformation task. Both children and adults were less…
Descriptors: Perspective Taking, Children, Preadolescents, Young Adults
Lagattuta, Kristin Hansen; Sayfan, Liat; Harvey, Christina – Child Development, 2014
Four- to 10-year-olds' and adults' (N = 263) ability to inhibit privileged knowledge and simulate a naïve perspective were examined. Participants viewed pictures that were then occluded aside from a small ambiguous part. They offered suggestions for how a naïve person might interpret the hidden pictures, as well as rated the probability…
Descriptors: Children, Adults, Inhibition, Perspective Taking
Surtees, Andrew D. R.; Apperly, Ian A. – Child Development, 2012
Children (aged 6-10) and adults (total N = 136) completed a novel visual perspective-taking task that allowed quantitative comparisons across age groups. All age groups found it harder to judge the other person's perspective when it differed from their own. This egocentric interference did not decrease with age, even though, overall, performance…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Perspective Taking, Children, Adults
Abrams, Dominic – Child Development, 2011
Does children's bias toward their own groups reflect egocentrism or social understanding? After being categorized as belonging to 1 of 2 fictitious groups, 157 six- to ten-year-olds evaluated group members and expressed preferences among neutral items. Children who expected the in-group to share their item preferences (egocentric social…
Descriptors: Student Attitudes, Perspective Taking, Group Dynamics, Psychological Patterns
Abrams, Dominic; Rutland, Adam; Pelletier, Joseph; Ferrell, Jennifer M. – Child Development, 2009
In Study 1, 167 English children aged 6-8 or 9-11 evaluated peer English or French soccer fans that were loyal or partially disloyal. In Study 2, 149 children aged 5-11 made judgments about generic inclusion norms between and within competitive groups. In both studies, children's understanding of intergroup inclusion/exclusion norms (group nous)…
Descriptors: Perspective Taking, Peer Groups, Group Dynamics, Norms
Fenning, Rachel M.; Baker, Bruce L.; Juvonen, Jaana – Child Development, 2011
This study examined parent-child emotion discourse, children's independent social information processing, and social skills outcomes in 146 families of 8-year-olds with and without developmental delays. Children's emergent social-cognitive understanding (internal state understanding, perspective taking, and causal reasoning and problem solving)…
Descriptors: Perspective Taking, Social Cognition, Problem Solving, Developmental Delays
Filippova, Eva; Astington, Janet Wilde – Child Development, 2008
This study describes the development of social reasoning in school-age children. An irony task is used to assess 5-, 7-, and 9-year-olds' (N = 72) and adults' (N = 24) recursive understanding of others' minds. Guttman scale analysis demonstrates that in order to understand a speaker's communicative intention, a child needs to recognize the…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Language Aptitude, Cognitive Development, Social Cognition

Krebs, Dennis; Gillmore, Janet – Child Development, 1982
Investigates the relationships among the first three stages of cognitive, role-taking, and moral development in both transitional and nontransitional subjects ages 5 to 14 years in order to determine whether the pattern of associations conformed more adequately to the "functional unity" model or to the "necessary but not…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Children, Cognitive Development, Developmental Stages

Brody, Gene H.; And Others – Child Development, 1982
Twenty-two subjects (school-age children, their younger siblings, and their best friends) were observed in their homes while playing a popular board game. Five roles were operationalized and observed: teacher, learner, manager, managee, and playmate. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Observation, Peer Relationship

Barrett, Justin L.; Richert, Rebekah A.; Driesenga, Amanda – Child Development, 2001
Three experiments examined assumption that children attribute human properties to nonhuman agents. Two- to 8-year-olds participated in false-belief tests concerning human and various nonhuman agents, including animals and God, and in a modified perspective-taking task including nonhuman agents. Results suggested that children do not consistently…
Descriptors: Beliefs, Childhood Attitudes, Children, Cognitive Development

Rosser, Rosemary A. – Child Development, 1983
A total of 120 children between four to eight years of age were administered four sets of visual perspective-taking tasks. Results supported the hypothesis that children's task competence would be a fraction of the number and type of spatial relationships embedded in the stimulus displays. (Author/MP)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Processes, Performance Factors

Dollinger, Stephen J.; And Others – Child Development, 1981
Studied children's attributions and evaluations concerning defense mechanisms used by other children. Children negatively evaluated the blame-externalizing defense of projection and viewed it as a masculine characteristic. The internalizing defense of self-blame was evaluated more positively and viewed as a feminine characteristic. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Attribution Theory, Children, Identification (Psychology), Perspective Taking

Cutrona, Carolyn E.; Feshbach, Seymour – Child Development, 1979
Examined individual differences among third-, fourth-, and fifth-grade children (N=59) in the tendency to remember and utilize dispositional as opposed to situational information in predicting and explaining the behavior of story characters. (JMB)
Descriptors: Aggression, Characterization, Children, Egocentrism

Kusche, Carol A.; Greenberg, Mark T. – Child Development, 1983
Evaluates the growth of social-cognitive knowledge in deaf and hearing children during the early- and middle-school years and assesses the relative importance of language in two domains of social cognition. In addition, separately examines the child's ability to evaluate the concepts of good and bad and to take another person's perspective. (RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Ability, Comparative Analysis

Enright, Robert D.; Lapsley, Daniel K. – Child Development, 1981
Examined judgments of intolerance given by children, adolescents, and adults toward disagreeing others. The evidence suggested that intolerance may be a lower level of reasoning in a social cognitive developmental progression. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Adults, Age Differences, Children
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2