Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 5 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 27 |
Metacognition | 27 |
Children | 9 |
Cognitive Processes | 7 |
Memory | 7 |
Preschool Children | 7 |
Cognitive Development | 6 |
Developmental Stages | 6 |
Elementary School Students | 6 |
Young Children | 6 |
Preschool Education | 5 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 27 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 27 |
Reports - Research | 27 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 2 |
Location
Canada | 2 |
China | 1 |
West Germany | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Zhao, Wanlin; Li, Baike; Shanks, David R.; Zhao, Wenbo; Zheng, Jun; Hu, Xiao; Su, Ningxin; Fan, Tian; Yin, Yue; Luo, Liang; Yang, Chunliang – Child Development, 2022
Recent studies established that making concurrent judgments of learning (JOLs) can significantly alter (typically enhance) memory itself--a "reactivity" effect. The current study recruited 190 Chinese children (M[subscript age] = 8.68 years; 101 female) in 2020 and 2021 to explore the reactivity effect on children's learning, its…
Descriptors: Evaluative Thinking, Memory, Metacognition, Children
Redshaw, Jonathan; Vandersee, Johanna; Bulley, Adam; Gilbert, Sam J. – Child Development, 2018
This study explored under what conditions young children would set reminders to aid their memory for delayed intentions. A computerized task requiring participants to carry out delayed intentions under varying levels of cognitive load was presented to 63 children (aged between 6.9 and 13.0 years old). Children of all ages demonstrated…
Descriptors: Cues, Memory, Intention, Prompting
Sobel, David M.; Letourneau, Susan M. – Child Development, 2018
It is widely believed that exploration is a mechanism for young children's learning. The present investigation examines preschoolers' beliefs about how learning occurs. We asked 3- to 5-year-olds to articulate how characters in a set of stories learned about a new toy. Younger preschoolers were more likely to overemphasize the role of characters'…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Beliefs, Inquiry, Learning Processes
Chevalier, Nicolas; Blaye, Agnès – Child Development, 2016
Emerging executive control supports greater autonomy and increasingly adaptive behavior during childhood. The present study addressed whether children's greater monitoring of how they engage control drives executive control development. Gaze position was recorded while twenty-five 6-year-olds and twenty-eight 10-year-olds performed a self-paced…
Descriptors: Metacognition, Executive Function, Child Behavior, Children
Danovitch, Judith H.; Fisher, Megan; Schroder, Hans; Hambrick, David Z.; Moser, Jason – Child Development, 2019
This study explored developmental and individual differences in intellectual humility (IH) among 127 children ages 6-8. IH was operationalized as children's assessment of their knowledge and willingness to delegate scientific questions to experts. Children completed measures of IH, theory of mind, motivational framework, and intelligence, and…
Descriptors: Neurology, Physiology, Social Cognition, Self Evaluation (Individuals)
Brownell, Celia A.; Nichols, Sara R.; Svetlova, Margarita; Zerwas, Stephanie; Ramani, Geetha – Child Development, 2010
Developments in very young children's topographic representations of their own bodies were examined. Sixty-one 20- and 30-month-old children were administered tasks that indexed the ability to locate specific body parts on oneself and knowledge of how one's body parts are spatially organized, as well as body-size knowledge and self-awareness. Age…
Descriptors: Topography, Self Concept, Age Differences, Toddlers
Jaswal, Vikram K.; Dodson, Chad S. – Child Development, 2009
Research on the development of metamemory has focused primarily on children's understanding of the variables that influence how likely a person is to remember something. But metamemory also involves an understanding of why people occasionally misremember things. In this study, 5- and 6-year-olds (N = 38) were asked to decide whether another…
Descriptors: Metacognition, Memory, Young Children, Age Differences

Lyon, Thomas D.; Flavell, John H. – Child Development, 1993
In two studies, four- but not three-year olds understood that (1) of two characters who saw an object, the one who waited longer before attempting to find it would not remember where it was; and (2) of two objects seen by a character, the object seen long ago would be forgotten and the object seen recently would be remembered. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Memory, Metacognition, Retention (Psychology)

Johnson, Edward A. – Child Development, 1997
Used stories involving self-deception, lying, and misleading appearances to examine 4- to 9-year olds' understanding of the relations between false belief, evidence, and epistemic responsibility. Found that younger children who understood false belief understood simpler types of deception but that understanding self-deceivers' epistemic…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Childhood Attitudes, Cognitive Development, Deception

Altshuler, Jennifer L.; Ruble, Diane N. – Child Development, 1989
Investigated awareness of strategies for coping with uncontrollable stress of 72 children of 5-12 years. Children mentioned approach strategies infrequently. Findings showed an age increase in the proportion of cognitive distraction strategies suggested. Behavioral distraction strategies were most frequently suggested by children of all ages. (RJC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Development, Children, Coping

Wilkinson, Louise Cherry; And Others – Child Development, 1984
Production and judgment tasks were used to investigate five- through eight-year-old children's metalinguistic awareness of pragmatic rules concerning direct and indirect requests for action and information. Results showed several effects for age of child and for type of request. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Discriminant Analysis, Elementary Education, Metacognition

Fabricius, William V.; And Others – Child Development, 1989
Examined concepts of mental activities involved in acts of knowing in 54 children of 8-10 years and adults. Ten-year-olds and adults judged memory involvement to be the most important relation among mental activities. Eight-year-olds judged comprehension and attention according to the involvement of visual or verbal information. (RJC)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Attention, Children

O'Neill, Daniela K.; And Others – Child Development, 1992
Three studies investigated the degree to which young children understand that the acquisition of certain types of knowledge depends on the modality of the sensory experience involved. Results suggest that an appreciation of the different types of knowledge our senses can provide develops between the ages of three and five years. (GLR)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Individual Development, Learning Modalities, Metacognition

Estes, David – Child Development, 1998
Four-year olds, 6-year olds, and adults were given a computer-game mental rotation task, but with no instructions on mental rotation or other mental activity. Reaction time patterns and verbal reports revealed that 6-year olds were comparable to adults in spontaneous use and subjective awareness of mental rotation. Four-year olds who referred to…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Metacognition

Mischel, Harriet Nerlove; Mischel, Walter – Child Development, 1983
Two studies traced the development of metacognitions about self-control in children from preschool through grade 6. Results indicated that children begin to understand two basic rules for effective delay of gratification by about the end of their fifth year. (Author/MP)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Processes, Delay of Gratification
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2