Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 4 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 13 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 48 |
Descriptor
Infant Behavior | 82 |
Infants | 64 |
Child Development | 34 |
Parent Child Relationship | 21 |
Mothers | 15 |
Neonates | 15 |
Cognitive Processes | 10 |
Child Rearing | 8 |
Research Methodology | 8 |
Cognitive Development | 7 |
Emotional Response | 7 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Adolph, Karen E. | 3 |
Als, Heidelise | 2 |
Fox, Nathan A. | 2 |
Mazuka, Reiko | 2 |
Needham, Amy | 2 |
Reddy, Vasudevi | 2 |
Richards, John E. | 2 |
Sameroff, Arnold J. | 2 |
Striano, Tricia | 2 |
Adrien, J. L. | 1 |
Angela M. Tomlin | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Evaluative | 82 |
Journal Articles | 79 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Information Analyses | 4 |
Reports - Descriptive | 3 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 3 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 19 |
Preschool Education | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Practitioners | 1 |
Researchers | 1 |
Location
Canada | 2 |
Sweden | 2 |
Finland | 1 |
Japan | 1 |
Netherlands | 1 |
New Zealand | 1 |
Ohio | 1 |
Pakistan | 1 |
South Africa | 1 |
South Carolina | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Brazelton Neonatal Assessment… | 6 |
Bayley Scales of Infant… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Hane, Amie Ashley; Fox, Nathan A.; Polak-Toste, Cindy; Ghera, Melissa M.; Guner, Bella M. – Developmental Psychology, 2006
To elucidate the differential saliency of infant emotions to mothers across interactive contexts, the authors examined the moderating role of observed infant affect during interactions with mother in the relation between maternal and laboratory-based ratings of infant temperament. Fifty-nine developmentally healthy 9-month-old infants were…
Descriptors: Personality, Infant Behavior, Infants, Mothers
Behne, Tanya; Carpenter, Malinda; Call, Josep; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Psychology, 2005
Infants experienced a female adult handing them toys. Sometimes, however, the transaction failed, either because the adult was in various ways unwilling to give the toy (e.g., she teased the child with it or played with it herself) or else because she was unable to give it (e.g., she accidentally dropped it). Infants at 9, 12, and 18 months of age…
Descriptors: Toys, Infants, Intention, Infant Behavior
Mash, Clay; Novak, Elizabeth; Berthier, Neil E.; Keen, Rachel – Developmental Psychology, 2006
Preferential-looking studies suggest that by 2 months of age, infants may have knowledge about some object properties, such as solidity. Manual search studies of toddlers examining these same concepts, however, have failed to provide evidence for the same understanding. Investigators have recently attempted to reconcile this disparity but failed…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Developmental Psychology, Reaction Time, Psychological Evaluation
Thomas, Michael S. C. – Infancy, 2004
Three developmental connectionist models simulate a purported shift from "featural" to "correlational" processing in infant categorization (models: Gureckis & Love, 2004/this issue; Shultz & Cohen, 2004/this issue; Westermann & Mareschal, 2004/this issue; empirical data: Cohen & Arthur, 2003; Younger, 1985; Younger & Cohen, 1986). In this article,…
Descriptors: Infants, Classification, Developmental Stages, Correlation
Callaghan, Tara C.; Rochat, Philippe; MacGillivray, Tanya; MacLellan, Crystal – Child Development, 2004
Social precursors to symbolic understanding of pictures were examined with 100 infants ages 6, 9, 12, 15, and 18 months. Adults demonstrated 1 of 2 stances toward pictures and objects (contemplative or manipulative), and then gave items to infants for exploration. For pictures, older infants (12, 15, and 18 months) emulated the adult's actions…
Descriptors: Infants, Socialization, Observational Learning, Pictorial Stimuli

Quinn, Paul C. – Psychological Record, 2005
Vidic and Haaf (2004) questioned the idea that infants use head information to categorize cats as distinct from dogs (Quinn & Eimas, 1996) and argued instead that the torso region is important. However, only null results were observed in the critical test comparisons between modified and unmodified stimuli. In addition, a priori preferences for…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Infants, Classification, Infant Behavior
Reeves, Christopher – Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 2006
This paper considers the meaning, reference and clinical relevance of Winnicott's concept of "riddance". Taking its starting point from the infant's behaviour in letting go the spatula, as described in his paper, "The observation of infants in a set situation", it explores his explanation of riddance activity in the context of…
Descriptors: Children, Anatomy, Psychotherapy, Child Behavior

Rea, Cathleen Althaus; And Others – American Annals of the Deaf, 1988
The interactive behaviors of eight deaf and eight hearing mothers and their hearing infants were examined during play. Compared to hearing mothers, deaf mothers produced fewer, shorter vocalizations, but more nonvocal initiations. The two groups of infants differed on frequency and duration of their vocalizations only at the 16-17 month age level.…
Descriptors: Deafness, Infant Behavior, Infants, Interaction
Ganea, Patricia A. – Child Development, 2005
How do infants come to understand references to absent objects? 14-month-old infants first learned a name for a novel toy, which was then placed out of view. The infants who listened to a story mentioning the nonvisible object, looked, pointed, and searched for it more often than did infants who heard a story using a different name. Their behavior…
Descriptors: Toys, Infants, Context Effect, Comprehension
Bremner, J. Gavin; Johnson, Scott P.; Slater, Alan; Mason, Uschi; Foster, Kirsty; Cheshire, Andrea; Spring, Joanne – Child Development, 2005
When an object moves behind an occluder and re-emerges, 4-month-old infants perceive trajectory continuity only when the occluder is narrow, raising the question of whether time or distance out of sight is the important constraining variable. One hundred and forty 4-month-olds were tested in five experiments aimed to disambiguate time and distance…
Descriptors: Infants, Infant Behavior, Perceptual Development, Visual Perception
Cowley, Stephen J.; Moodley, Sheshni; Fiori-Cowley, Agnese – Mind, Culture, and Activity, 2004
The article examines how infants are first permeated by culture. Building on Thibault (2000), semiogenesis is traced to the joint activity of primary intersubjectivity. Using an African example, analysis shows how--at 14 weeks--an infant already uses culturally specific indicators of "what a caregiver wants." Human predispositions and…
Descriptors: Infants, Culture, Semiotics, Mothers
Berger, Sarah E.; Adolph, Karen E.; Lobo, Sharon A. – Child Development, 2005
This study examined whether 16-month-old walking infants take the material composition of a handrail into account when assessing its effectiveness as a tool to augment balance. Infants were encouraged to cross from one platform to another via bridges of various widths (10, 20, 40cm) with either a wobbly (foam or latex) or a wooden handrail…
Descriptors: Child Development, Physical Activities, Infant Behavior, Toddlers
Needham, Amy; Cantlon, Jessica F.; Ormsbee Holley, Susan M. – Cognitive Psychology, 2006
The current research investigates infants' perception of a novel object from a category that is familiar to young infants: key rings. We ask whether experiences obtained outside the lab would allow young infants to parse the visible portions of a partly occluded key ring display into one single unit, presumably as a result of having categorized it…
Descriptors: Infants, Investigations, Visual Perception, Classification

Strauss, Milton E.; Rourke, Daniel L. – Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1978
Discusses differences in results of factor analyses of ten diverse samples which have been studied using the Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (NBAS). Concludes that a single common factor structure accounts for the intercorrelations among NBAS items. (Author/BH)
Descriptors: Child Development, Factor Analysis, Infant Behavior, Infants
Reddy, Vasudevi; Trevarthen, Colwyn – Zero to Three (J), 2004
Reddy and Trevarthen explore what we can learn from emotionally engaging with babies. Theirs is a different approach from 20th-century psychology, in which doubt and detachment play a role in discerning other people's feelings and thoughts. Instead, the authors suggest that emotions are the key to psychological engagement. When interacting with an…
Descriptors: Infants, Infant Behavior, Emotional Response, Emotional Development