Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 23 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Aleman Lopez, P. A. | 1 |
Andreas Nehring | 1 |
Anton-Fos, G. M. | 1 |
Apperly, Ian A. | 1 |
Beck, Sarah R. | 1 |
Bobos, Georgeana | 1 |
Bosse, Tibor | 1 |
Brannon, Elizabeth M. | 1 |
Buder, Jurgen | 1 |
Chappell, Jackie | 1 |
Chen, Wen | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 27 |
Reports - Evaluative | 27 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 8 |
Elementary Education | 4 |
Postsecondary Education | 4 |
High Schools | 2 |
Middle Schools | 2 |
Secondary Education | 2 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Grade 6 | 1 |
Grade 7 | 1 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Marco Reith; Andreas Nehring – Journal of Chemical Education, 2022
Scientific reasoning competencies enable students to engage in scientific inquiry through a wide range of methods, for example by asking questions, formulating hypotheses, and planning, carrying out, and evaluating experiments. Laboratories are particularly suitable to foster these competencies in higher education. In order to give lab educators…
Descriptors: Thinking Skills, Undergraduate Students, Inquiry, Laboratory Experiments
Yang, Yisi; Zhang, Yan; Xiong, Xujie; Zhang, Wanju; Chen, Wen; Ge, Shiping – Journal of Chemical Education, 2021
Project-based learning (PJBL) is student-centered and teamwork educational activity that prepares students for the challenge of real-world problems. This kind of instructional method is highly recommended by the Accreditation Board for Engineering and Technology (ABET).This paper describes an authentic project that focuses on the preparation of…
Descriptors: Student Projects, Active Learning, Science Instruction, Learning Activities
Vansieleghem, Nancy – Educational Philosophy and Theory, 2014
Philosophy seems to have gained solid ground in the hearts and minds of educational researchers and practitioners. We critique Philosophy for Children as an experimental programme aimed at improving children's thinking capacity, by questioning the concept of critique itself. What does it mean when an institutional framework like the school…
Descriptors: Philosophy, Children, Critical Thinking, Thinking Skills
Schwind, Christina; Buder, Jurgen; Cress, Ulrike; Hesse, Friedrich W. – Computers & Education, 2012
The Web is a perfect backdrop for opinion formation as a multitude of different opinions is publicly available. However, the different opinions often remain unexploited: Learners prefer preference-consistent over preference-inconsistent information, a phenomenon called confirmation bias. Two experiments were designed to test whether technologies…
Descriptors: Opinions, Creative Thinking, Internet, Experiments
Zhao, Jiaying; Crupi, Vincenzo; Tentori, Katya; Fitelson, Branden; Osherson, Daniel – Cognition, 2012
Bayesian orthodoxy posits a tight relationship between conditional probability and updating. Namely, the probability of an event "A" after learning "B" should equal the conditional probability of "A" given "B" prior to learning "B". We examine whether ordinary judgment conforms to the orthodox view. In three experiments we found substantial…
Descriptors: Probability, Thinking Skills, Correlation, Experiments
ErEl, Hadas; Meiran, Nachshon – Cognition, 2011
Rule finding is an important aspect of human reasoning and flexibility. Previous studies associated rule finding "failure" with past experience with the test stimuli and stable personality traits. We additionally show that rule finding performance is severely impaired by a mindset associated with applying an instructed rule. The mindset was…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Personality Traits, Stimuli, Cognitive Processes
Son, Ji Y.; Smith, Linda B.; Goldstone, Robert L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
The practice of learning from multiple instances seems to allow children to learn about relational structure. The experiments reported here focused on two issues regarding relational learning from multiple instances: (a) what kind of perceptual situations foster such learning and (b) how particular object properties, such as complexity and…
Descriptors: Theory Practice Relationship, Generalization, Children, Thinking Skills
Beck, Sarah R.; Apperly, Ian A.; Chappell, Jackie; Guthrie, Carlie; Cutting, Nicola – Cognition, 2011
Tool making evidences intelligent, flexible thinking. In Experiment 1, we confirmed that 4- to 7-year-olds chose a hook tool to retrieve a bucket from a tube. In Experiment 2, 3- to 5-year-olds consistently failed to innovate a simple hook tool. Eight-year-olds performed at mature levels. In contrast, making a tool following demonstration was easy…
Descriptors: Experiments, Children, Thinking Skills, Age Differences
Moore, J. Christopher – European Journal of Physics Education, 2012
University and high school students not pursuing a science, technology, engineering, and/or mathematics (STEM) course of study demonstrate less developed scientific reasoning than their STEM-based peers. Previous studies show that the majority of non-STEM students can be classified as either concrete operational or transitional reasoners in…
Descriptors: Nonmajors, College Science, Scientific Literacy, Thinking Skills
Poddiakov, Alexander – International Journal of Early Years Education, 2011
Combinatorial abilities are fundamental to experimental thinking. The aim of this work was to design didactic objects that will stimulate preschoolers' experimental thinking and to study young children's thinking in relation to these objects. Six heuristic rules for the design of didactic objects are specified, and the responses of 623 children…
Descriptors: Early Intervention, Young Children, Experiments, Thinking Skills
Sierpinska, Anna; Bobos, Georgeana; Pruncut, Andreea – Educational Studies in Mathematics, 2011
This paper gives an account of a teaching experiment on absolute value inequalities, whose aim was to identify characteristics of an approach that would realize the potential of the topic to develop theoretical thinking in students enrolled in prerequisite mathematics courses at a large, urban North American university. The potential is…
Descriptors: Mathematics Education, Institutional Characteristics, Prerequisites, North Americans
Zamora, L. Lahuerta; Anton-Fos, G. M.; Aleman Lopez, P. A.; Martin Algarra, R. V. – Journal of Chemical Education, 2008
Skepticism is one of the cornerstones of scientific learning. Some pseudosciences in domains such as astronomy or pharmacy use a host of issues in everyday life as pretexts for work in the classroom (e.g., astrology) or laboratory (e.g., homeopathy). Chemistry also offers opportunities to promote skeptical thinking in students. Commercial devices…
Descriptors: Magnets, Water, Science Instruction, Science Experiments
Grimberg, Bruna Irene; Hand, Brian – International Journal of Science Education, 2009
The purpose of this study was to reconstruct writers' reasoning process as reflected in their written texts. The codes resulting from the text analysis were related to cognitive operations, ranging from simple to more sophisticated ones. The sequence of the cognitive operations as the text unfolded represents the writer's cognitive pathway at the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Content Analysis, Grade 7, Middle School Students
Wilson, Helen; Mant, Jenny – School Science Review, 2011
Questionnaires were completed by 5044 12-year-old pupils in Oxfordshire state schools and initially used to identify classes where the pupils were more positive and enthusiastic about their science lessons than the majority. The teachers of these classes were identified and the views of their pupils as to what happens in their science lessons…
Descriptors: Science Instruction, Science Teachers, Foreign Countries, Questionnaires
Windschitl, Paul D.; Conybeare, Daniel; Krizan, Zlatan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2008
Above-average and below-average effects appear to be common and consistent across a variety of judgment domains. For example, several studies show that individual items from a high- (low-) quality set tend to be rated as better (worse) than the other items in the set (e.g., E. E. Giladi & Y. Klar, 2002). Experiments in this article demonstrate…
Descriptors: Probability, Resource Allocation, Experiments, Thinking Skills
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2