NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 10 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Santone, Elizabeth; Crothers, Laura M.; Kolbert, Jered B.; Miravalle, Joseph – Journal of School Counseling, 2020
The social information processing (SIP) model, which involves a sequence of six cognitive processing steps, is frequently used by researchers to understand proactive and reactive aggression in youth; however, there has been little discussion in the literature regarding the application of the SIP model in school counseling. This article presents a…
Descriptors: Counseling Techniques, Aggression, Cognitive Processes, Bullying
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stroebe, Wolfgang; van Koningsbruggen, Guido M.; Papies, Esther K.; Aarts, Henk – Psychological Review, 2013
Theories of eating regulation often attribute overweight to a malfunction of homeostatic regulation of body weight. With the goal conflict model of eating, we present a new perspective that attributes the difficulty of chronic dieters (i.e., restrained eaters) in regulating their food intake to a conflict between 2 incompatible goals--namely,…
Descriptors: Behavior Theories, Body Weight, Nutrition, Eating Habits
Miwa, Kazuhisa; Kojima, Kazuaki; Terai, Hitoshi – International Association for Development of the Information Society, 2014
Tutoring systems provide students with various types of on-demand and context-sensitive hints. Students are required to consciously adapt their help-seeking behavior, proactively seek help in some situations, and solve problems independently without supports in other situations. We define the latter behavior as stoic behavior in hint seeking. In…
Descriptors: Help Seeking, Student Behavior, Cues, Goal Orientation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Craighero, Laila; Leo, Irene; Umilta, Carlo; Simion, Francesca – Cognition, 2011
The central role of sensory-motor representations in cognitive functions is almost universally accepted. However, determining the link between motor execution and its sensory counterpart and when, during ontogenesis, this link originates are still under investigation. The aim of the present study is to investigate whether at birth this link is…
Descriptors: Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Neonates, Goal Orientation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sargent, Jesse; Dopkins, Stephen; Philbeck, John; Modarres, Reza – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2008
Human spatial representations of object locations in a room-sized environment were probed for evidence that the object locations were encoded relative not just to the observer (egocentrically) but also to each other (allocentrically). Participants learned the locations of 4 objects and then were blindfolded and either (a) underwent a succession of…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, College Students, Memory, Experimental Psychology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Shute, Valerie J. – Review of Educational Research, 2008
This article reviews the corpus of research on feedback, with a focus on formative feedback--defined as information communicated to the learner that is intended to modify his or her thinking or behavior to improve learning. According to researchers, formative feedback should be nonevaluative, supportive, timely, and specific. Formative feedback is…
Descriptors: Student Characteristics, Formative Evaluation, Feedback (Response), Evaluation Methods
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kallie, Christopher S.; Schrater, Paul R.; Legge, Gordon E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2007
Walking without vision results in veering, an inability to maintain a straight path that has important consequences for blind pedestrians. In this study, the authors addressed whether the source of veering in the absence of visual and auditory feedback is better attributed to errors in perceptual encoding or undetected motor error. Three…
Descriptors: Physical Activities, Cues, Blindness, Orientation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dreisbach, Gesine – Brain and Cognition, 2006
Adaptive action in a constantly changing environment requires the ability to maintain intentions and goals over time and to flexibly switch between these goals in response to significant changes. Dreisbach and Goschke (2004) argued that positive affect modulates these antagonistic control demands in favor of a more flexible but also more…
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Cognitive Ability, Goal Orientation, Positive Reinforcement
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Boekaerts, Monique; Cascallar, Eduardo – Educational Psychology Review, 2006
In this article, we address four main questions, including: What is self-regulated learning for? What key strategies do students need to guide and direct their own learning process? What cues in the learning environment trigger self-regulation strategies? What can teachers do to help student to self-regulate their learning, motivation, and effort…
Descriptors: Self Management, Learning Processes, Learning Strategies, Cues
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wentzel, Kathryn R.; Filisetti, Laurence; Looney, Lisa – Child Development, 2007
Peer- and teacher-reported prosocial behavior of 339 6th-grade (11-12 years) and 8th-grade (13-14 years) students was examined in relation to prosocial goals, self-processes (reasons for behavior, empathy, perspective taking, depressive affect, perceived competence), and contextual cues (expectations of peers and teachers). Goal pursuit…
Descriptors: Perspective Taking, Empathy, Prosocial Behavior, Early Adolescents