NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 10 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Liebal, Katja; Haun, Daniel B. M. – International Journal of Developmental Science, 2012
The aim of this essay is to elucidate the relevance of cross-species comparisons for the investigation of human behavior and its development. The focus is on the comparison of human children and another group of primates, the non-human great apes, with special attention to their cognitive skills. Integrating a comparative and developmental…
Descriptors: Developmental Psychology, Comparative Analysis, Experimental Psychology, Thinking Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Fischhoff, Baruch; Gonzalez, Roxana M.; Lerner, Jennifer S.; Small, Deborah A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2012
The authors examined the evolution of cognitive and emotional responses to terror risks for a nationally representative sample of Americans between late 2001 and late 2002. Respondents' risk judgments changed in ways consistent with their reported personal experiences. However, they did not recognize these changes, producing hindsight bias in…
Descriptors: Priming, Psychological Studies, Emotional Response, Risk
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Beck, Sarah R.; Apperly, Ian A.; Chappell, Jackie; Guthrie, Carlie; Cutting, Nicola – Cognition, 2011
Tool making evidences intelligent, flexible thinking. In Experiment 1, we confirmed that 4- to 7-year-olds chose a hook tool to retrieve a bucket from a tube. In Experiment 2, 3- to 5-year-olds consistently failed to innovate a simple hook tool. Eight-year-olds performed at mature levels. In contrast, making a tool following demonstration was easy…
Descriptors: Experiments, Children, Thinking Skills, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Namy, Laura L.; Clepper, Lauren E. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
Comparison of perceptually similar exemplars from an object category encourages children to overlook compelling perceptual similarities and use relational and functional properties more relevant for taxonomic categorization. This article investigates whether showing children a contrasting object that is perceptually similar but out of kind serves…
Descriptors: Classification, Experimental Psychology, Child Psychology, Comparative Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Piazza, Jared; Bering, Jesse M.; Ingram, Gordon – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Two child groups (5-6 and 8-9 years of age) participated in a challenging rule-following task while they were (a) told that they were in the presence of a watchful invisible person ("Princess Alice"), (b) observed by a real adult, or (c) unsupervised. Children were covertly videotaped performing the task in the experimenter's absence. Older…
Descriptors: Cheating, Individual Differences, Child Psychology, Experimental Psychology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Brainerd, C. J.; Holliday, R. E.; Reyna, V. F.; Yang, Y.; Toglia, M. P. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
Do the emotional valence and arousal of events distort children's memories? Do valence and arousal modulate counterintuitive age increases in false memory? We investigated those questions in children, adolescents, and adults using the Cornell/Cortland Emotion Lists, a word list pool that induces false memories and in which valence and arousal can…
Descriptors: Semantics, Word Lists, Memory, Experimental Psychology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gao, Xiaoqing; Maurer, Daphne – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
Using 20 levels of intensity, we measured children's thresholds to discriminate the six basic emotional expressions from neutral and their misidentification rates. Combined with the results of a previous study using the same method ("Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102" (2009) 503-521), the results indicate that by 5 years of age,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Development, Emotional Response, Nonverbal Communication
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Beran, Michael J.; Johnson-Pynn, Julie S.; Ready, Christopher – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2008
The performances of 4- and 5-year-olds and rhesus monkeys were compared using a computerized task for quantity assessment. Participants first learned two quantity anchor values and then responded to intermediate values by classifying them as similar to either the large anchor or the small anchor. Of primary interest was an assessment of where the…
Descriptors: Measures (Individuals), Geometric Concepts, Comparative Analysis, Computer Uses in Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Deruelle, Christine; Rondan, Cecilie; Gepner, Bruno; Tardif, Carole – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2004
Two experiments were designed to investigate possible abnormal face processing strategies in children with autistic spectrum disorders. A group of 11 children with autism was compared to two groups of normally developing children matched on verbal mental age and on chronological age. In the first experiment, participants had to recognize faces on…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Spatial Ability, Autism, Asperger Syndrome
Mussen, Paul – 1977
Research in the development of prosocial behavior in children is better done by naturalistic than by experimental methods. The choice of what problems to address in social science research depends upon value commitments. The ultimate criterion for evaluating research in the behavioral sciences must be social relevance and utility. The dominant…
Descriptors: Children, Experimental Psychology, Individual Development, Models