Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Serial Learning | 9 |
Experimental Psychology | 3 |
Learning Processes | 3 |
Recall (Psychology) | 3 |
Comparative Analysis | 2 |
Correlation | 2 |
Memorization | 2 |
Memory | 2 |
Word Lists | 2 |
Alphabets | 1 |
Articulation (Speech) | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 3 |
Developmental Science | 1 |
Early Childhood Research… | 1 |
Journal of Experimental… | 1 |
Neuropsychologia | 1 |
Psychological Bulletin | 1 |
Research in Developmental… | 1 |
Author
Anaya, Esperanza M. | 1 |
Biswas, Asit B. | 1 |
Bowles, Ryan B. | 1 |
Brown, Gordon D. A. | 1 |
Capaldi, E. J. | 1 |
Conway, Christopher M. | 1 |
Cumming, Nick | 1 |
Fisher, Laura | 1 |
Furniss, Frederick | 1 |
Gebauer, Guido F. | 1 |
Gumber, Rohit | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Evaluative | 9 |
Information Analyses | 2 |
Education Level
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Germany | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Shin, Yun Kyoung; Proctor, Robert W.; Capaldi, E. J. – Psychological Bulletin, 2010
A framework for action planning, called "ideomotor theory," suggests that actions are represented by their perceivable effects. Thus, any activation of the effect image, either endogenously or exogenously, will trigger the corresponding action. We review contemporary studies relating to ideomotor theory in which researchers have investigated…
Descriptors: Psychophysiology, Evidence, Stimuli, Sensory Experience
Conway, Christopher M.; Pisoni, David B.; Anaya, Esperanza M.; Karpicke, Jennifer; Henning, Shirley C. – Developmental Science, 2011
Deaf children with cochlear implants (CIs) represent an intriguing opportunity to study neurocognitive plasticity and reorganization when sound is introduced following a period of auditory deprivation early in development. Although it is common to consider deafness as affecting hearing alone, it may be the case that auditory deprivation leads to…
Descriptors: Deafness, Disadvantaged Environment, Assistive Technology, Auditory Perception
Mayor-Dubois, C.; Maeder, P.; Zesiger, P.; Roulet-Perez, E. – Neuropsychologia, 2010
We investigated procedural learning in 18 children with basal ganglia (BG) lesions or dysfunctions of various aetiologies, using a visuo-motor learning test, the Serial Reaction Time (SRT) task, and a cognitive learning test, the Probabilistic Classification Learning (PCL) task. We compared patients with early (less than 1 year old, n=9), later…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Neurological Impairments, Pathology, Patients
Furniss, Frederick; Biswas, Asit B.; Gumber, Rohit; Singh, Niraj – Research in Developmental Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2011
The behavioural phenotype of velocardiofacial syndrome (VCFS), one of the most common human multiple anomaly syndromes, includes developmental disabilities, frequently including intellectual disability (ID) and high risk of diagnosis of psychotic disorders including schizophrenia. VCFS may offer a model of the relationship between ID and risk of…
Descriptors: Mental Retardation, Schizophrenia, Developmental Disabilities, Serial Learning
Gebauer, Guido F.; Mackintosh, Nicholas J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2007
The hypothesis that performance on implicit learning tasks is unrelated to psychometric intelligence was examined in a sample of 605 German pupils. Performance in artificial grammar learning, process control, and serial learning did not correlate with various measures of intelligence when participants were given standard implicit instructions.…
Descriptors: Psychometrics, Serial Learning, Intelligence, Intelligence Tests
Kvavilashvili, Lia; Fisher, Laura – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2007
The present research examined self-reported rehearsal processes in naturalistic time-based prospective memory tasks (Study 1 and 2) and compared them with the processes in event-based tasks (Study 3). Participants had to remember to phone the experimenter either at a prearranged time (a time-based task) or after receiving a certain text message…
Descriptors: Motivation, Cues, Memorization, Memory
Hulme, Charles; Neath, Ian; Stuart, George; Shostak, Lisa; Surprenant, Aimee M.; Brown, Gordon D. A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2006
The authors report 2 experiments that compare the serial recall of pure lists of long words, pure lists of short words, and lists of long or short words containing just a single isolated word of a different length. In both experiments for pure lists, there was a substantial recall advantage for short words; the isolated words were recalled better…
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Experimental Psychology, Serial Learning, Recall (Psychology)
Page, Mike P. A.; Cumming, Nick; Norris, Dennis; Hitch, Graham J.; McNeil, Alan M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2006
In 5 experiments, a Hebb repetition effect, that is, improved immediate serial recall of an (unannounced) repeating list, was demonstrated in the immediate serial recall of visual materials, even when use of phonological short-term memory was blocked by concurrent articulation. The learning of a repeatedly presented letter list in one modality…
Descriptors: Learning Processes, Serial Learning, Recall (Psychology), Visual Aids
Justice, Laura M.; Pence, Khara; Bowles, Ryan B.; Wiggins, Alice – Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 2006
This study tested four complementary hypotheses to characterize intrinsic and extrinsic influences on the order with which preschool children learn the names of individual alphabet letters. The hypotheses included: (a) "own-name advantage," which states that children learn those letters earlier which occur in their own names, (b) the…
Descriptors: Hypothesis Testing, Alphabets, Influences, Preschool Children