Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 4 |
Descriptor
Experiments | 6 |
Semantics | 6 |
Language Processing | 5 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Cues | 2 |
Adults | 1 |
Attention | 1 |
Attention Control | 1 |
Children | 1 |
Classification | 1 |
Comparative Analysis | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Clahsen, Harald | 1 |
Felser, Claudia | 1 |
Hoffmann, Joachim | 1 |
Kiesel, Andrea | 1 |
Kunde, Wilfried | 1 |
Moss, Helen | 1 |
Paivio, Allan | 1 |
Piai, Vitoria | 1 |
Randall, Billi | 1 |
Reynvoet, Bert | 1 |
Roelofs, Ardi | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Opinion Papers | 6 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Adult Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Germany | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Paivio, Allan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2013
Kousta, Vigliocco, Del Campo, Vinson, and Andrews (2011) questioned the adequacy of dual coding theory and the context availability model as explanations of representational and processing differences between concrete and abstract words. They proposed an alternative approach that focuses on the role of emotional content in the processing of…
Descriptors: Epistemology, Semantics, Language Processing, Psychological Patterns
Roelofs, Ardi; Piai, Vitoria; Schriefers, Herbert – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
E. Dhooge and R. J. Hartsuiker (2010) reported experiments showing that picture naming takes longer with low- than high-frequency distractor words, replicating M. Miozzo and A. Caramazza (2003). In addition, they showed that this distractor-frequency effect disappears when distractors are masked or preexposed. These findings were taken to refute…
Descriptors: Attention Control, Attention, Experiments, Semantics
Taylor, Kirsten I.; Salamoura, Angeliki; Randall, Billi; Moss, Helen; Tyler, Lorraine K. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2008
The conceptual structure account of semantic memory (CSA; L. K. Tyler & H. E. Moss, 2001) claims that feature correlation (the degree to which features co-occur) and feature distinctiveness (the number of concepts in which a feature occurs) interact with domains of knowledge (e.g., living vs. nonliving) such that the distinctive features of…
Descriptors: Distinctive Features (Language), Semantics, Memory, Correlation
Van Opstal, Filip; Reynvoet, Bert; Verguts, Tom – Cognition, 2005
In their original report [Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. "Cognition," 88, 223-242] maintain that ''unconscious stimuli [do not] owe their impact [...] to automatic semantic categorization'' (p.223), and instead propose the action-trigger theory of unconscious priming. In a…
Descriptors: Semantics, Classification, Language Processing, Criticism
Kunde, Wilfried; Kiesel, Andrea; Hoffmann, Joachim – Cognition, 2005
We have recently argued that unconscious numerical stimuli might activate responses by a match with prespecified action trigger codes (action trigger account) rather than by semantic prime processing (elaborate processing account). [Van Opstal, F., Reynvoet, B., and Verguts, T. (2005). How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Experiments, Semantics, Language Processing
Clahsen, Harald; Felser, Claudia – Applied Psycholinguistics, 2006
The core idea that we argued for in the target article was that grammatical processing in a second language (L2) is fundamentally different from grammatical processing in one's native (first) language (L1). Our major source of evidence for this claim comes from experimental psycholinguistic studies investigating morphological and syntactic…
Descriptors: Evidence, Language Dominance, Cues, Semantics