NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 91 to 105 of 117 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Chermak, Gail D.; O'Connell, Vickie I. – Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1981
Twenty normal children were administered three tests of auditory sequential memory. A Pearson product-moment correlation of .50 and coefficients of determination showed all but one relationship to be nonsignificant and predictability between pairs of scores to be poor. (Author)
Descriptors: Auditory Perception, Children, Comparative Testing, Correlation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Johnson, G. J. – Psychological Review, 1991
An associative model of serial learning is described based on the assumption that the effective stimulus for a serial-list item is generated by adaptation-level coding of the item's ordinal position. How the model can generate predictions of aspects of serial-learning data is illustrated. (SLD)
Descriptors: Association (Psychology), Associative Learning, Coding, Difficulty Level
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Star, Jon R. – Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 2005
In this article, I argue for a renewed focus in mathematics education research on procedural knowledge. I make three main points: (1) The development of students' procedural knowledge has not received a great deal of attention in recent research; (2) one possible explanation for this deficiency is that current characterizations of conceptual and…
Descriptors: Mathematics Education, Concept Formation, Scientific Methodology, Sequential Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Justice, Laura M.; Pence, Khara; Bowles, Ryan B.; Wiggins, Alice – Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 2006
This study tested four complementary hypotheses to characterize intrinsic and extrinsic influences on the order with which preschool children learn the names of individual alphabet letters. The hypotheses included: (a) "own-name advantage," which states that children learn those letters earlier which occur in their own names, (b) the…
Descriptors: Hypothesis Testing, Alphabets, Influences, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bebko, James M. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1984
Tests 64 deaf students from oral and total communication settings to examine whether a deficiency in spontaneous strategy use accounts for their verbal short-term memory performance. Spontaneous rehearsal of both deaf samples seemed to emerge later than the hearing sample's and was inefficiently implemented and less effective in mediating recall…
Descriptors: Deafness, Learning Strategies, Oral Communication Method, Recall (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Ashman, Adrian – Australia and New Zealand Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 1983
The utility of a model of cognition for developing the coding strategies of mentally retarded persons was assessed with 20 moderately and severely retarded residents (mean age 15 years) of an institution. Correlational analyses confirmed the sequential processing nature of the training tasks, and the relationship between coding and language tests.…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Cognitive Processes, Exceptional Child Research, Intervention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Chuah, Y. M. Lisa; Maybery, Murray T. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1999
Used a variance-partitioning procedure to identify age-related and age-invariant components of verbal and spatial memory span in 6- to 12-year olds. Concluded that verbal and spatial short-term memory appear to rely on similar processes when serial recall is required and that development in span is closely tied to increases in processing speed.…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Articulation (Speech), Children, Cognitive Processes
Test, David W.; And Others – Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps (JASH), 1988
This study examined the use of supported employment to provide competitive janitorial work experience for a 19-year-old severely mentally handicapped student. Training consisted of a combination of total task presentation and an individualized prompting hierarchy. Results demonstrated acquisition and maintenance of all skills at 100 percent…
Descriptors: Behavior Chaining, Case Studies, Job Skills, On the Job Training
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fazio, Barbara B. – Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1996
Mathematical performance by 14 first- and second-grade children with specific language impairments (SLI) indicated difficulty with mathematical tasks that required immediate response. Findings suggest that storage and/or retrieval of rote sequential material is difficult for children with SLI. (Author/PB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Followup Studies, Language Impairments, Mathematics Achievement
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Baltes, Paul B.; Kliegl, Reinhold – Developmental Psychology, 1992
Tested the ability of 19 older adults, given additional training in a mental imagination technique, to approach the performance of 16 younger adults on serial word recall tasks. Results indicated that negative age differences in older adults' performance were substantial, resistant to extensive practice, and applicable to all subjects. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Encoding (Psychology), Foreign Countries
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gulya, Michele; Sweeney, Becky; Rovee-Collier, Carolyn – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1999
Three experiments demonstrated that increasing the length of a mobile serial list impaired 6-month olds' memory for serial order. Findings indicated that the primacy effect was absent on a 24-hour delayed recognition test and was exhibited on a reactivation test, adding to growing evidence that young infants possess two functionally distinct…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Infant Behavior, Infants, Long Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zheng, Robert; Zhou, Bei – Educational Technology & Society, 2006
This study investigated the impact of recency effect on multiple rule-based problem solving in an interactive multimedia environment. Forty-five college students were recruited and assigned to two groups: synchronized and unsynchronized interactive multimedia groups based on their spatial ability score. Results show that students in the…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Problem Solving, Spatial Ability, Multimedia Instruction
Levin, Joel R.; And Others – Educational Communication and Technology: A Journal of Theory, Research, and Development, 1983
Students learned the numerical order of 14 U.S. Presidents through either a complex mnemonic strategy or their own technique. Performance pattern differences were detected between the groups, chiefly serial position profiles produced by those using their own techniques, and slower response time of subjects using mnemonic strategies. (Author/MBR)
Descriptors: Junior High School Students, Junior High Schools, Learning Processes, Learning Strategies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Mason, Mildred – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1982
Three experiments report additional evidence that it is a mistake to account for all interletter effects solely in terms of sensory variables. These experiments attest to the importance of structural variables such as retina location, array size, and ordinal position. (Author/PN)
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Cognitive Processes, Eye Fixations, Higher Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Richardson, John T. E. – Cognitive Psychology, 1979
A system of precategorized acoustic storage has accounted for the recency effect obtained in the immediate serial recall of sequences of digits, consonants, or syllables. Four experiments in recall of word sequences investigated fit to this model. A system of postcategorical lexical storage was concluded to explain the results. (Author/RD)
Descriptors: Auditory Stimuli, Cognitive Processes, Foreign Countries, Higher Education
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7  |  8