Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 4 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 11 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 42 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 231 |
Descriptor
Verbal Stimuli | 478 |
Visual Stimuli | 91 |
Cognitive Processes | 67 |
Foreign Countries | 58 |
Children | 56 |
Verbal Communication | 55 |
Memory | 52 |
Recall (Psychology) | 51 |
Autism | 49 |
Comparative Analysis | 46 |
Adults | 45 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Greer, R. Douglas | 5 |
Kodak, Tiffany | 4 |
Plavnick, Joshua B. | 4 |
Ackerman, Brian P. | 3 |
Barnes-Holmes, Dermot | 3 |
Carr, James E. | 3 |
Dymond, Simon | 3 |
Eikeseth, Svein | 3 |
Jones, Dylan M. | 3 |
Miguel, Caio F. | 3 |
O'Reilly, Mark F. | 3 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 21 |
Practitioners | 15 |
Teachers | 9 |
Location
Canada | 7 |
Netherlands | 6 |
United Kingdom (England) | 6 |
Australia | 4 |
Germany | 4 |
China | 2 |
Japan (Tokyo) | 2 |
New York (New York) | 2 |
New Zealand | 2 |
Norway | 2 |
Alabama | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Equal Rights Amendment | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 1 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 1 |
Does not meet standards | 1 |

Koroscik, Judith S.; And Others – Studies in Art Education, 1985
Verbal contextual information affected photography and nonphotography students' performance on semantic retention tests. For example, correct titles aided the formation and retention of accurate memories, while erroneous titles misled students into remembering meanings that had relatively little to do with what was actually pictured in the…
Descriptors: Art Education, Association Measures, Context Clues, Educational Research

Penner, Kandace A.; Williams, William N. – Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1982
The relationship between sign and verbal learning was explored using 10 severely mentally retarded adults. They were taught color labels in sign, verbal, or sign and verbal groups. Sign labels tended to be learned more efficiently; combined sign and verbal training improved verbal learning but not sign learning. (Author/CM)
Descriptors: Institutionalized Persons, Learning Processes, Oral Language, Severe Mental Retardation

Cutler, Anne; Fodor, Jerry A. – Cognition, 1979
Reaction time to detect a phoneme target in a sentence was faster when the target-containing word formed part of the semantic focus of the sentence. Sentence understanding was facilitated by rapid identification of focused information. Active search for accented words can be interpreted as a search for semantic focus. (Author/RD)
Descriptors: Adults, Higher Education, Linguistic Performance, Listening Comprehension
Durso, Francis T.; Johnson, Marcia K. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1979
Subjects named or categorized a picture preceded sometime earlier by itself or by its verbal label, as well as a word preceded by itself or a pictorial counterpart. Pictures clearly profited more when the task was naming, whereas words profited more when subjects performed a categorization task. (Author/GDC)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Higher Education, Language Processing, Learning Experience

Baron-Cohen, Simon; And Others – Child Development, 1997
Two studies of toddlers and children with autism, mentally handicapped children, and normal toddlers examined whether autistic toddlers used Speaker's Direction of Gaze (SDG) strategy or less powerful Listener's Direction of Gaze (LDG) strategy to learn a word for a novel object. Results suggest autistic toddlers are insensitive to speaker's gaze…
Descriptors: Autism, Cognitive Processes, Context Clues, Language Processing

Bandy, Kenneth E.; Young, Jon I. – Information Technology, Learning, and Performance Journal, 2002
Case study groups (n=106) were randomly assigned to face-to-face, chat, or group support systems conditions; half of each group received structural primers. Ditransitive structural frequency counts indicated that the primer significantly shifted learning style. Complexity was significantly greater in the group support system treatment. (14…
Descriptors: Cognitive Style, Communication (Thought Transfer), Computer Assisted Instruction, Decision Support Systems

Mize, Jacquelyn; Ladd, Gary W. – Developmental Psychology, 1988
Findings provide tentative support for the hypothesis that strategy information derived from enactive interviews is more predictive of children's behavior in peer situations than is information derived from the verbal interview method. This suggests that preschool children automatically access cognitive scripts for behavior in social interactions.…
Descriptors: Assertiveness, Comparative Analysis, Interpersonal Competence, Peer Acceptance

Craig, Chie H. – Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1988
Examined were the influences of three conditions of predictability on word-recognition performance by 30 normally hearing listeners. Significant differences in word recognition were observed among high-predictability phrases, low-predictability phrases, and a constant predictability-neutral carrier phrase, using three specific signal-to-babble…
Descriptors: Acoustic Phonetics, Adults, Auditory Perception, Comparative Analysis

Siegel, Linda S. – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1994
Examined relationships among working memory, memory span, and reading skills in children and adults. Found that working memory and short-term memory skills develop through adolescence, but working memory skills show declines in adulthood. Age-related declines in memory appear to be related to the task's processing demands, which may affect the…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development

Colombo, J.; And Others – Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1995
Examined the possibility that auditory stimuli with properties of adult-to-infant speech are more detectable in a noisy ambient environment than ones that resemble adult-to-adult speech. Findings suggests that properties that characterize adult-to-infant speech may compensate for young infants' low-frequency deficits and therefore facilitate the…
Descriptors: Auditory Perception, Auditory Stimuli, Caregiver Speech, Communication Research

Ullman, Sarah E.; Knight, Raymond A. – Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 1992
Using police reports and court testimonies, examined women's resistance strategies to rape. Results indicated that 85 percent of the women who resisted with physical force were responding to the offender's initiated violence. Women reacting with physical aggression to violent attack were more likely to avoid rape than were nonresisting women. (RJM)
Descriptors: Aggression, Antisocial Behavior, Overt Response, Personality Traits

Lipkens, Regina; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1993
Tested a normally developing child several times between 16 and 27 months of age for his ability to derive the relations between stimuli. Found that the child derived "mutual entailment" relations and showed "nonverbal exclusion" as early as 17 months. "Combinatorial entailment" relations and "verbal…
Descriptors: Child Development, Cognitive Development, Language Acquisition, Longitudinal Studies

Rodriguez, Carlos Xavier; Webster, Peter Richard – Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 1997
Examines children's verbal responses to music listening by interviewing 33 children in kindergarten through fifth-grade. Explains that three judges categorized their responses. Indicates judge consensus over the form and content of responses; age tendencies resulted in 3 questions; similarities between development of verbal responses to music and…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Art, Art Expression, Child Development

Lancioni, Giulio E.; O'Reilly, Mark F.; Oliva, Doretta – International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 2001
Three adults with intellectual and visual disabilities were taught vocationally relevant tasks through the use of a self-operated verbal instruction system. Data showed that during the training all participants achieved percentages of correct task performance exceeding 90. Use of instructional clusters was effective in maintaining high levels of…
Descriptors: Adults, Educational Media, Mental Retardation, Performance Factors
Pexman, Penny M.; Glenwright, Melanie; Krol, Andrea; James, Tammy – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2005
Around 5 or 6 years of age, children begin to recognize that speakers who make ironic remarks do not believe what they literally say, but children of the same age do not show appreciation for the humor function of irony (Dews et al., 1996; Harris & Pexman, 2003). We investigated 7- to 10-year-old children's interpretations of verbal irony and…
Descriptors: Humor, Figurative Language, Child Psychology, Psychological Studies