Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 4 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 21 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 53 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 141 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Donders, Jacobus | 5 |
Dieterich, Daniel J. | 3 |
Kamhi, Alan G. | 3 |
Leutner, Detlev | 3 |
Mayer, Richard E. | 3 |
Scruggs, Thomas E. | 3 |
Aisen, Paul S. | 2 |
Anstey, Kaarin J. | 2 |
Baddeley, Alan | 2 |
Baddeley, Alan D. | 2 |
Behm, Richard H. | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 30 |
Practitioners | 27 |
Teachers | 17 |
Location
Australia | 7 |
Netherlands | 7 |
Canada | 6 |
California | 5 |
Israel | 5 |
India | 4 |
United Kingdom | 4 |
United Kingdom (England) | 4 |
Belgium | 2 |
China | 2 |
Egypt | 2 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 1 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 1 |
Mayer, Richard E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1979
The idea was explored that different subjects may use qualitatively different encoding strategies with the same information presented within the same experimental situation. The premise was also studied that different strategies may be evoked by subtle differences in advance instructions or in the context of the task. (Author/MH)
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Higher Education, Learning Processes, Learning Theories
Byrne, Brian – Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1979
In open-ended and forced-choice situations, subjects were asked to interpret phrases containing superficially incompatible adjective pairs. The results demonstrate that English speakers implicitly understand proposed linguistic principles underlying prenominal adjective ordering. (SW)
Descriptors: Adjectives, Grammar, Language Research, Linguistic Theory

Skuy, Mervyn; And Others – Gifted Child Quarterly, 1990
The study with 99 13 to 18-year-old Black students in the Soweto (South Africa) Gifted Child Program indicated that 50-58 percent of the variance in performance was accounted for by 2 subtests (the Verbal Analogies Test and the Organiser) of the Learning Potential Assessment Device and the Similarities subtests of the Wechsler scales. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Blacks, Cognitive Processes, Foreign Countries, Gifted Disadvantaged

Schooler, Jonathan W.; Engstler-Schooler, Tonya Y. – Cognitive Psychology, 1990
The hypothesis that describing a visual memory can result in recoding interference was investigated in a series of 6 experiments with 518 college students. Collective results were consistent with the hypothesis; verbalizing memory can produce a verbally biased memory representation that can interfere with the original visual memory. (SLD)
Descriptors: College Students, Higher Education, Memory, Recognition (Psychology)

Bowers, Kenneth S.; And Others – Cognitive Psychology, 1990
A total of 308 undergraduates performed 2 word tasks and a gestalt closure task in a study of intuition. Subjects could respond discriminately to coherence they could not identify and were led by this perception to form a hunch or hypothesis. Clues to coherence evidently activate problem-solving networks. (SLD)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Discovery Processes, Higher Education, Intuition

Vorster, Jan – Language Sciences, 1988
Longitudinal studies of the application of a paraphrasing model to 18- to 28-month-olds indicated that mean length of utterance was significantly correlated with realized and paraphrased frequencies of several linguistic items in the subjects' corpora. The model was productive for examining children's corpora of speech and the linguistic…
Descriptors: Child Language, Language Acquisition, Longitudinal Studies, Oral Language
Fletcher, Kathryn L.; Bray, Norman W. – American Journal on Mental Retardation, 1995
Comparison of external memory strategies in 31 children (ages 11 and 17) with mild mental retardation and 64 children without mental retardation found no differences between children with mental retardation and their age peers in frequency of use of object-oriented strategies. For all groups, external strategies were used more frequently than…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Cognitive Processes, Learning Strategies, Memory

Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1992
In three experiments, adults and 30-month-old children (1) selected a novel object as the referent for a novel term; (2) extended the new word to another exemplar; and (3) allowed the new word to preempt another novel label from applying to the just-named object. (BC)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Classification, Comparative Analysis

Cohen, Ronald L. – Intelligence, 1994
A case is made for the construction of nomothetic theories that can also explain individual differences. The discussion uses examples from the memory area and presents an approach to memory that explains individual findings and individual differences in the context of a single model. (SLD)
Descriptors: Encoding (Psychology), Individual Differences, Memory, Models

Booth, Gregory D.; Cutietta, Robert A. – Journal of Research in Music Education, 1991
Presents results of a test of nonmusic majors' ability to remember song titles. Suggests that, although the process of categorization seems to be a basic function of perception, appropriate categorization needs to be learned. Supports the view that music learning, like verbal learning, involves a categorization of stimuli based on holistic…
Descriptors: Classification, Music Appreciation, Music Education, Nonmajors

Baddeley, Alan; Gathercole, Susan; Papagno, Costanza – Psychological Review, 1998
A review of studies of word learning by normal adults and children, neuropsychological patients, and special development populations provides evidence that the phonological loop plays a crucial role in learning the novel phonological forms of new words, suggesting that the phonological loop stores unfamiliar sound patterns while more permanent…
Descriptors: Adults, Children, Language Acquisition, Learning Disabilities

Cole, Peter G.; Pheng, Lee Chee – International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 1998
Thirty children with partial sight and 30 children with unimpaired sight (all 8- to 12-years old) received either verbal-mediation or visual-mediation training regimes. The verbal mediation training was superior to visual mediation training on a problem-solving task for both children with partial sight and children without visual impairments.…
Descriptors: Children, Instructional Effectiveness, Partial Vision, Problem Solving

Ohta, Masataka – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 1987
Analysis of the conceptions of spatial relations, size comparisons, and gesture imitations of 16 autistic children (ages 6-14 years) with IQs over 70 indicated an inability to acquire concepts of size comparison and spatial relationships through verbal instructions, suggesting a cognitive deficit of impaired symbolic-representational functioning.…
Descriptors: Autism, Children, Cognitive Processes, Concept Formation

Deacon, Joseph R.; Konarski, Edward A., Jr. – Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1987
Results of a study comparing the outcome of a reinforcement (do only) procedure with correspondence (say/do) training indicated no apparent differences in generalization between two groups of mentally retarded adults (N=12). Rule-governed behavior, rather than verbal regulation of behavior, may best account for behavior changes seen in…
Descriptors: Adults, Behavior Modification, Contingency Management, Generalization

Jacka, Brian – Journal of Educational Research, 1985
An ambiguity in Gagne and Briggs' model of instructional design was investigated using treatments which contrasted verbal expository and guided discovery forms of presentation. Analysis found the guided discovery method as effective as the expository method on all measures. Implications are discussed. (Author/MT)
Descriptors: Discovery Learning, Elementary Education, Grade 6, Instructional Design