NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing all 14 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Matthias Grünke; Isabel Gürcay; Janine Bracht; Alina Jochims; Matthias Schulden; Anne Barwasser; Ellen Duchaine – International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 2024
The ability to recognize and name the sounds of alphabet letters is a crucial prerequisite for students as they embark on their journey to learn how to read. Regrettably, some children face significant challenges in this area. In this single-case multiple baseline study, we utilized mnemonic pictures to facilitate the memorization of the…
Descriptors: Phoneme Grapheme Correspondence, Mnemonics, Pictorial Stimuli, Grade 1
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ehri, Linnea C. – Reading Teacher, 2022
A hallmark of skilled reading is recognizing written words automatically from memory by sight. How beginning readers attain this skill is explained. They must acquire foundational knowledge, including phonemic segmentation, grapheme-phoneme knowledge, decoding, and spelling skills. When these skills are applied, spellings of words become bonded to…
Descriptors: Phonics, Phonemic Awareness, Spelling, Phoneme Grapheme Correspondence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
O'Leary, Robin; Ehri, Linnea C. – Reading Research Quarterly, 2020
The authors examined whether exposing young students to spellings as they learn proper names would facilitate memory for the spoken names when tested without the spellings present (i.e., orthographic facilitation), whether emergent readers with letter knowledge would show this effect, and whether phonemic segmentation (PS) training would enhance…
Descriptors: Orthographic Symbols, Memory, Naming, Nouns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Esposito, John – Reading Matrix: An International Online Journal, 2017
The primary impediment to literacy in Japanese is kanji proficiency. The ostensible reason kanji present such a formidable challenge, especially for the second language learner, is the combined effect of their quantity and complexity. Research into the cognitive processing of logographic characters, however, indicates that the main obstacle to…
Descriptors: Mnemonics, Written Language, Phoneme Grapheme Correspondence, Second Language Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Nicholas, Maria; McKenzie, Sophie; Wells, Muriel A. – Journal of Education and Learning, 2017
When integrated within a holistic literacy program, phonics applications can be used in classrooms to facilitate students' self-directed learning of letter-sound knowledge; but are they designed to allow for such a purpose? With most phonics software applications making heavy use of image cues, this project has more specifically investigated…
Descriptors: Phonics, Computer Software, Computer Uses in Education, Elementary School Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Beal-Alvarez, Jennifer S.; Lederberg, Amy R.; Easterbrooks, Susan R. – Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2012
We examined acquisition of grapheme-phoneme correspondences by 4 deaf and hard-of-hearing preschoolers using instruction from a curriculum designed specifically for this population supplemented by Visual Phonics. Learning was documented through a multiple baseline across content design as well as descriptive analyses. Preschoolers who used sign…
Descriptors: Phoneme Grapheme Correspondence, Deafness, Hearing Impairments, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Shmidman, Adina; Ehri, Linnea – Scientific Studies of Reading, 2010
Can embedded mnemonics ease the task of learning a foreign alphabet? English-speaking preschoolers (N = 36, M = 5;2 years) were taught 10 Hebrew letter-sound relations. Experimental letters were learned with mnemonics that embedded letter shapes in drawings of objects whose shapes resembled the letters and whose English names began with the…
Descriptors: Phonics, Mnemonics, Learning Strategies, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
DiLorenzo, Kim E.; Rody, Carlotta A.; Bucholz, Jessica L.; Brady, Michael P. – Preventing School Failure, 2011
The authors explored "Itchy's Alphabet" as an innovative approach to teaching letter-sound connections through multisensory cues. This is the initial demonstration of this instructional method for increasing students' sublexical skills (letter identification, letter-sound knowledge, and phonological awareness). To examine the effectiveness of the…
Descriptors: Early Reading, Orthographic Symbols, Phonological Awareness, Kindergarten
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rosenthal, Julie; Ehri, Linnea C. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2008
In 2 experiments, the authors examined whether spellings improve students' memory for pronunciations and meanings of new vocabulary words. Lower socioeconomic status minority 2nd graders (M = 7 years 7 months; n = 20) and 5th graders (M = 10 years 11 months; n = 32) were taught 2 sets of unfamiliar nouns and their meanings over several learning…
Descriptors: Sentences, Spelling, Nouns, Pronunciation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
de Graaff, Saskia; Verhoeven, Ludo; Bosman, Anna M. T.; Hasselman, Fred – British Journal of Educational Psychology, 2007
Background: The conclusion from a vast literature on literacy acquisition is that letter knowledge is one of the best predictors of literacy development. The question of the best way to teach children letter sounds has not, as yet, been answered satisfactorily. Aims: The aim of this study was the evaluation of a computer training program using…
Descriptors: Mnemonics, Early Childhood Education, Kindergarten, Preschool Evaluation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Wilshire, Carolyn E. – Language and Speech, 1999
Two experiments explored the tongue-twister paradigm, which involves reciting a word string several times over at a fast rate, using a task variation that minimizes articulatory and mnemonic load. The task was found to elicit good rates of "pure" articulatory errors. Two features had a significant error-reducing effect: repeated…
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Encoding (Psychology), Error Analysis (Language), Error Patterns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Ehri, Linnea C.; And Others – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1984
Two experiments evaluated whether picture mnemonics help prereaders learn letter-sound associations. Pictures integrating the associations were compared with disassociated pictures and a no-picture control group. Children taught with integrated mnemonics learned more letter-sound associations and more letter-picture associations than the other two…
Descriptors: Association (Psychology), Integrated Activities, Mnemonics, Phoneme Grapheme Correspondence
Hoogeveen, Frans R.; And Others – Education and Training in Mental Retardation, 1987
Using seven trainable mentally-retarded students, aged 10-19, the study assessed the efficacy of an action mnemonic procedure for teaching letter-sound correspondences. The subjects were successfully trained to emit appropriate action-sounds in the presence of pictures incorporating the target letter, with picture-prompts being gradually faded to…
Descriptors: Elementary Secondary Education, Intervention, Mnemonics, Moderate Mental Retardation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fulk, Barbara M.; And Others – Learning Disability Quarterly, 1997
Using a multiple baseline across students design, the effects of an integrated picture mnemonic strategy on the letter-sound acquisition of three transitional first-grade students with special needs were investigated. Follow-up data indicated the method was an effective instructional technique and that results were maintained over time. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Beginning Reading, Decoding (Reading), Grade 1, Instructional Effectiveness