NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 10 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Avi Benozio; Bailey R. House; Michael Tomasello – Developmental Psychology, 2024
A foundational mechanism underlying human cooperation is reciprocity. In the context of repeated interactions with others, it is not always clear the degree to which in-kind responses reflect responsiveness to partners' prior behaviors ("reactive" responses), an interest unrelated to the partner ("nonreactive" responses), or…
Descriptors: Child Development, Young Children, Gender Differences, Cultural Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stengelin, Roman; Ball, Rabea; Maurits, Luke; Kanngiesser, Patricia; Haun, Daniel B. M. – Developmental Science, 2023
Researchers commonly use puppets in development science. Amongst other things, puppets are employed to reduce social hierarchies between child participants and adult experimenters akin to peer interactions. However, it remains controversial whether children treat puppets like real-world social partners in these settings. This study investigated…
Descriptors: Young Children, Puppetry, Interaction, Imitation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Grosse, Gerlind; Simon, Antonia; Soemer, Alexander; Schönfeld, Romy; Barth, Stefanie; Linde, Nadine – International Journal of Early Childhood, 2022
How does teacher-child interaction quality in early child care and education settings influence the development of social-emotional skills in children at around two to three years of age? We measured the quality of interactions on the side of the child care teachers (N = 9, CLASS Toddler) and assessed self-regulatory skills through an individual…
Descriptors: Teacher Student Relationship, Interaction, Memory, Social Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Elisa Oppermann; Sabine Blaurock; Lysann Zander; Yvonne Anders – Early Education and Development, 2024
Research Findings: The study examined the development of children's social-emotional problems between 2019 (T1) and winter 2021/22 (T2) during the COVID-19 pandemic, as well as the role of children's home and preschool learning environments. The sample included 228 German children ages 3-7 years at T1 (M[subscript Age] = 5.13, SD = 0.79, 46%…
Descriptors: Social Emotional Learning, COVID-19, Pandemics, Family Environment
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Messinger, Daniel S.; Prince, Emily B.; Zheng, Minzhang; Martin, Katherine; Mitsven, Samantha G.; Huang, Shengda; Stölzel, Tanja; Johnson, Neil; Rudolph, Udo; Perry, Lynn K.; Laursen, Brett; Song, Chaoming – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2019
Human observations can only capture a portion of ongoing classroom social activity, and are not ideal for understanding how children's interactions are spatially structured. Here we demonstrate how social interaction can be investigated by modeling automated continuous measurements of children's location and movement using a commercial system…
Descriptors: Interaction, Play, Peer Relationship, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wurm, Moritz F.; Artemenko, Christina; Giuliani, Daniela; Schubotz, Ricarda I. – Developmental Psychology, 2017
Actions are recognized faster and with higher accuracy when they take place in their typical environments. It is unclear, however, when contextual cues from the environment become effectively exploited during childhood and whether contextual integration interacts with other factors such as children's perceptual or motor experience with an action.…
Descriptors: Young Children, Recognition (Psychology), Context Effect, Familiarity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mammen, Maria; Köymen, Bahar; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Psychology, 2018
Moral justifications work, when they do, by invoking values that are shared in the common ground of the interlocutors. We asked 3- and 5-year-old peer dyads (N = 144) to identify and punish norm transgressors. In the moral condition, the transgressor violated a moral norm (e.g., by stealing); in the social rules condition, she/he violated a…
Descriptors: Moral Values, Young Children, Peer Relationship, Social Attitudes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mayer, Andreas; Träuble, Birgit – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2015
Previous cross-cultural research using false-belief tasks has explored whether children's theory of mind develops synchronously across cultures. Success on false-belief tasks is usually interpreted as an important indicator of children's mental state understanding, but inconsistent findings have led to questions regarding the interpretation of…
Descriptors: Cross Cultural Studies, Foreign Countries, Theory of Mind, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Köymen, Bahar; Schmerse, Daniel; Lieven, Elena; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Psychology, 2014
In 2 studies, we investigated how peers establish a "referential pact" to call something, for example, a "cushion" versus a "pillow" (both equally felicitous). In Study 1, pairs of 4-and 6-year-old German-speaking peers established a referential pact for an artifact, for example, a "woman's shoe," in a…
Descriptors: Peer Relationship, Young Children, Age Differences, Language Usage
Beller, Simone – Bernard van Leer Foundation (NJ1), 2008
The ways in which children learn a language--be it their mother tongue or their second language--can have a strong influence on their success in school. Researchers in linguistics and early child development have tried to determine the factors that can help and hinder language acquisition in young children, with some conflicting results. In this…
Descriptors: Socioeconomic Status, Disadvantaged Youth, Young Children, Migrant Children