Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 2 |
Foreign Countries | 2 |
Language Acquisition | 2 |
Tone Languages | 2 |
Word Recognition | 2 |
Animals | 1 |
Audio Equipment | 1 |
Auditory Stimuli | 1 |
Bilingualism | 1 |
Cognitive Mapping | 1 |
Color | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Developmental Psychology | 2 |
Author
Demuth, Katherine | 1 |
Rattanasone, Nan Xu | 1 |
Singh, Leher | 1 |
Tang, Ping | 1 |
Wewalaarachchi, Thilanga D. | 1 |
Yuen, Ivan | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
China | 2 |
Singapore | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Singh, Leher; Wewalaarachchi, Thilanga D. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
Most children learn a language such as Mandarin Chinese that uses lexical tone to communicate meaning. This study aimed to examine the phonological specificity of tone representations in monolingual and bilingual learners of Mandarin. Two age-groups were tested: toddlers (2.5 to 3.5 years) and preschoolers (4 to 5 years; N = 80). Using a…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Native Speakers, Tone Languages, Word Recognition
Tang, Ping; Yuen, Ivan; Demuth, Katherine; Rattanasone, Nan Xu – Developmental Psychology, 2023
Contrastive focus, conveyed by prosodic cues, marks important information. Studies have shown that 6-year-olds learning English and Japanese can use contrastive focus during online sentence comprehension: focus used in a "contrastive context" facilitates the identification of a target referent (speeding up processing), whereas focus used…
Descriptors: Mandarin Chinese, Suprasegmentals, Intonation, Prediction