Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 10 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 27 |
Descriptor
Classification | 27 |
Visual Stimuli | 27 |
College Students | 15 |
Foreign Countries | 14 |
Undergraduate Students | 11 |
Cognitive Processes | 10 |
Cues | 7 |
Experimental Psychology | 7 |
Memory | 7 |
Statistical Analysis | 7 |
Color | 6 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 21 |
Journal of the Experimental… | 2 |
HOW | 1 |
International Journal of… | 1 |
International Journal of… | 1 |
Journal of Experimental… | 1 |
Author
Giesen, Carina | 2 |
Rothermund, Klaus | 2 |
Adamczyk, Markie N. | 1 |
Arntzen, Erik | 1 |
Ball, B. Hunter | 1 |
Bell, Raoul | 1 |
Blair, Mark R. | 1 |
Boukrina, Olga | 1 |
Buchner, Axel | 1 |
Bugg, Julie M. | 1 |
Bundesen, Claus | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 27 |
Reports - Research | 27 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 27 |
Postsecondary Education | 27 |
Audience
Location
Germany | 4 |
Georgia | 2 |
New York | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
California | 1 |
Canada | 1 |
Colombia | 1 |
Denmark | 1 |
Indiana | 1 |
Missouri | 1 |
Norway | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Smith, J. David; Jackson, Brooke N.; Adamczyk, Markie N.; Church, Barbara A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2022
Categorization researchers have long debated the possibility of multiple category-learning systems. The need persists for paradigms that dissociate explicit-declarative category-learning processes (featuring verbalizable category rules) from implicit-procedural processes (featuring stimulus-response associations lying beneath declarative…
Descriptors: Classification, Concept Formation, Perception, Learning Processes
Roark, Casey L.; Lehet, Matthew I.; Dick, Frederic; Holt, Lori L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2022
Category learning is fundamental to cognition, but little is known about how it proceeds in real-world environments when learners do not have instructions to search for category-relevant information, do not make overt category decisions, and do not experience direct feedback. Prior research demonstrates that listeners can acquire task-irrelevant…
Descriptors: Classification, Learning Processes, Schemata (Cognition), Decision Making
Isolating Component Processes of Posterror Slowing with the Psychological Refractory Period Paradigm
Steinhauser, Marco; Ernst, Benjamin; Ibald, Kevin W. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
Posterror slowing (PES) refers to an increased response time following errors. While PES has traditionally been attributed to control adjustments, recent evidence suggested that PES reflects interference. The present study investigated the hypothesis that control and interference represent 2 components of PES that differ with respect to their time…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Interference (Learning), Cognitive Processes, Classification
McDaniel, Mark A.; Cahill, Michael J.; Bugg, Julie M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
How does orthographic distinctiveness affect recall of structured (categorized) word lists? On one theory, enhanced item-specific information (e.g., more distinct encoding) in concert with robust relational information (e.g., categorical information) optimally supports free recall. This predicts that for categorically structured lists,…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Recall (Psychology), Word Lists, Cognitive Processes
Tu, Hsiao-Wei; Diana, Rachel A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
In recognition memory, "recollection" is defined as retrieval of the context associated with an event, whereas "familiarity" is defined as retrieval based on item strength alone. Recent studies have shown that conventional recollection-based tasks, in which context details are manipulated for source memory assessment at test,…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Memory, Recall (Psychology), Cognitive Processes
Cao, Rui; Nosofsky, Robert M.; Shiffrin, Richard M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
In short-term-memory (STM)-search tasks, observers judge whether a test probe was present in a short list of study items. Here we investigated the long-term learning mechanisms that lead to the highly efficient STM-search performance observed under conditions of consistent-mapping (CM) training, in which targets and foils never switch roles across…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Recall (Psychology), Item Response Theory, Learning Processes
Chen, Stephanie Y.; Ross, Brian H.; Murphy, Gregory L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Category information is used to predict properties of new category members. When categorization is uncertain, people often rely on only one, most likely category to make predictions. Yet studies of perception and action often conclude that people combine multiple sources of information near-optimally. We present a perception-action analog of…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Classification, Logical Thinking, Prediction
Holden, Mark P.; Newcombe, Nora S.; Shipley, Thomas F. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
Memories for spatial locations often show systematic errors toward the central value of the surrounding region. The Category Adjustment (CA) model suggests that this bias is due to a Bayesian combination of categorical and metric information, which offers an optimal solution under conditions of uncertainty (Huttenlocher, Hedges, & Duncan,…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Memory, Models, Task Analysis
Giesen, Carina; Rothermund, Klaus – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Stimulus-response (S-R) episodes are formed whenever a response is executed in close temporal proximity to a stimulus. Subsequent stimulus repetition will retrieve the episode from memory, reactivating the previous response. Whereas many research findings attest to the flexibility of representing stimulus features, only little is known about the…
Descriptors: Semantics, Motor Reactions, Task Analysis, Memory
Kurtz, Kenneth J.; Boukrina, Olga; Gentner, Dedre – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
We investigated the effect of co-presenting training items during supervised classification learning of novel relational categories. Strong evidence exists that comparison induces a structural alignment process that renders common relational structure more salient. We hypothesized that comparisons between exemplars would facilitate learning and…
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Classification, Experiments, Undergraduate Students
Gambi, Chiara; Van de Cavey, Joris; Pickering, Martin J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
In 4 experiments we showed that picture naming latencies are affected by beliefs about the task concurrently performed by another speaker. Participants took longer to name pictures when they believed that their partner concurrently named pictures than when they believed their partner was silent (Experiments 1 and 4) or concurrently categorized the…
Descriptors: Interference (Learning), Barriers, Pictorial Stimuli, Naming
Giesen, Carina; Rothermund, Klaus – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Even an irrelevant distractor stimulus is integrated into event files. Subsequently repeating the distractor triggers retrieval of the event file; however, an unresolved issue concerns the question of "what" is retrieved by the distractor. While recent studies predominantly assume that the distractor retrieves the previous response, it…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Interference (Learning), Responses, Priming
Walker, Peter – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Lexical sound symbolism in language appears to exploit the feature associations embedded in cross-sensory correspondences. For example, words incorporating relatively high acoustic frequencies (i.e., front/close rather than back/open vowels) are deemed more appropriate as names for concepts associated with brightness, lightness in weight,…
Descriptors: Sensory Integration, Intonation, Suprasegmentals, Phonology
Pashler, Harold; Mozer, Michael C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
Training that uses exaggerated versions of a stimulus discrimination (fading) has sometimes been found to enhance category learning, mostly in studies involving animals and impaired populations. However, little is known about whether and when fading facilitates learning for typical individuals. This issue was explored in 7 experiments. In…
Descriptors: Experiments, Classification, Cognitive Processes, Cues
Effects of a Meaningful, a Discriminative, and a Meaningless Stimulus on Equivalence Class Formation
Fields, Lanny; Arntzen, Erik; Nartey, Richard; Eilifsen, Christoffer – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2012
Thirty college students attempted to form three 3-node 5-member equivalence classes under the simultaneous protocol. After concurrent training of AB, BC, CD, and DE relations, all probes used to assess the emergence of symmetrical, transitive, and equivalence relations were presented for two test blocks. When the A-E stimuli were all abstract…
Descriptors: College Students, Visual Stimuli, Classification, Behavior
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2