Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 5 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Adults | 9 |
Word Recognition | 9 |
Grade 4 | 8 |
Elementary School Students | 5 |
Grade 5 | 5 |
Foreign Countries | 4 |
Grade 6 | 4 |
Age Differences | 3 |
Grade 2 | 3 |
Grade 3 | 3 |
Language Processing | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Acha, Joana | 1 |
Adlof, Suzanne | 1 |
Blythe, Hazel I. | 1 |
Casalis, Séverine | 1 |
Dandy, Jennifer | 1 |
Escalonilla, Ainoa | 1 |
Frishkoff, Gwen | 1 |
García, Seila | 1 |
Geudens, Astrid | 1 |
Gomez, Pablo | 1 |
Hasenäcker, Jana | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 9 |
Journal Articles | 8 |
Education Level
Grade 4 | 9 |
Elementary Education | 6 |
Grade 5 | 5 |
Intermediate Grades | 5 |
Grade 2 | 4 |
Grade 3 | 4 |
Grade 6 | 4 |
Early Childhood Education | 3 |
Middle Schools | 3 |
Primary Education | 3 |
Grade 7 | 2 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Gates MacGinitie Reading Tests | 1 |
Stanford Achievement Tests | 1 |
Wechsler Individual… | 1 |
Wide Range Achievement Test | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Tânia Fernandes; Sofia Velasco; Isabel Leite – Developmental Science, 2024
Discrimination of reversible mirrored letters (e.g., d and b) poses a challenge when learning to read as it requires overcoming "mirror invariance," an evolutionary-old perceptual tendency of processing mirror images as equivalent. The present study investigated "when," in reading development, mirror-image discrimination…
Descriptors: Grade 2, Grade 3, Grade 4, Grade 5
Lázaro, Miguel; Illera, Víctor; Acha, Joana; Escalonilla, Ainoa; García, Seila; Sainz, Javier S. – Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2018
The role of morphological processing has been shown to be highly relevant in learning to read. However, there is little evidence on the processing of derivational suffixes from a developmental perspective. The aim of this study is to assess the developmental emergence of suffixes as meaningful processing units in word recognition. To that aim, 96…
Descriptors: Morphology (Languages), Word Recognition, Suffixes, Spanish
Pagán, Ascensión; Blythe, Hazel I.; Liversedge, Simon P. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Although previous research has shown that letter position information for the first letter of a parafoveal word is encoded less flexibly than internal word beginning letters (Johnson, Perea & Rayner, 2007; White et al., 2008), it is not clear how positional encoding operates over the initial trigram in English. This experiment explored the…
Descriptors: Adults, Children, Experimental Psychology, Reading Processes
Hasenäcker, Jana; Schröter, Pauline; Schroeder, Sascha – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
The developmental trajectory of the use of morphemes is still unclear. We investigated the emergence of morphological effects on visual word recognition in German in a large sample across the complete course of reading acquisition in elementary school. To this end, we analyzed lexical decision data on a total of 1,152 words and pseudowords from a…
Descriptors: Morphemes, German, Morphology (Languages), Word Recognition
Quémart, Pauline; Casalis, Séverine – Scientific Studies of Reading, 2014
We report two experiments that investigated whether phonological and/or orthographic shifts in a base word interfere with morphological processing by French 3rd, 4th, and 5th graders and adults (as a control group) along the time course of visual word recognition. In both experiments, prime-target pairs shared four possible relationships:…
Descriptors: Phonology, Orthographic Symbols, Morphology (Languages), Language Processing
Adlof, Suzanne; Frishkoff, Gwen; Dandy, Jennifer; Perfetti, Charles – Grantee Submission, 2016
Word learning can build the high-quality word representations that support skilled reading and language comprehension. According to the partial knowledge hypothesis, words that are partially known, a.k.a. "frontier words" (Durso & Shore, 1991), may be good targets for instruction precisely because they are already familiar. However,…
Descriptors: Semantics, Familiarity, Adults, Children
Perea, Manuel; Panadero, Victoria; Moret-Tatay, Carmen; Gomez, Pablo – Learning and Instruction, 2012
Recent research has demonstrated that slight increases of inter-letter spacing have a positive impact on skilled readers' recognition of visually presented words. In the present study, we examined whether this effect generalises to young normal readers and readers with developmental dyslexia, and whether increased inter-letter spacing affects the…
Descriptors: Evidence, Dyslexia, Word Recognition, Grade 4
Van den Broeck, Wim; Geudens, Astrid; van den Bos, Kees P. – Developmental Psychology, 2010
This article presents empirical evidence challenging the received wisdom that a nonword-reading deficit is a characteristic trait of disabled readers. On the basis of 2 large-scale empirical studies using the reading-level match design, we argue that a nonword-reading deficit is the consequence of normal developmental differences in word-specific…
Descriptors: Reading Difficulties, Reading Skills, Disabilities, Developmental Stages
Ventura, Paulo; Kolinsky, Regine; Pattamadilok, Chotiga; Morais, Jose – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2008
The influence of orthography on children's online auditory word recognition was studied from the end of Grade 4 to the end of Grade 9 by examining the orthographic consistency effect in auditory lexical decision. Fourth-graders showed evidence of a widespread influence of orthography in their spoken word recognition system; words with rimes that…
Descriptors: Word Recognition, Grade 4, Grade 9, Influences