Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Adults | 2 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Theory of Mind | 2 |
Verbal Communication | 2 |
Affective Behavior | 1 |
Child Development | 1 |
Congruence (Psychology) | 1 |
Context Effect | 1 |
Correlation | 1 |
Criticism | 1 |
Cues | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 2 |
Grade 3 | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Intermediate Grades | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Location
Japan | 1 |
North America | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Hayashi, Hajimu; Shiomi, Yuki – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2015
This study examined whether children understand that people selectively conceal or express emotion depending upon the context. We prepared two contexts for a verbal display task for 70 first-graders, 80 third-graders, 64 fifth-graders, and 71 adults. In both contexts, protagonists had negative feelings because of the behavior of the other…
Descriptors: Psychological Patterns, Grade 1, Grade 3, Grade 5
Nilsen, Elizabeth S.; Glenwright, Melanie; Huyder, Vanessa – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2011
Incongruity between a positive statement and a negative context is a cue to verbal irony. Two studies examined whether school-age children and adults recognized that listeners require knowledge of context to detect irony. Specifically, the studies investigated whether participants could inhibit their own context knowledge to appropriately gauge…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Cues, Verbal Communication, Theory of Mind