Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1 |
Descriptor
Native Speakers | 2 |
Phonology | 2 |
Chinese | 1 |
Correlation | 1 |
Duplication | 1 |
Foreign Countries | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Jargon | 1 |
Language Variation | 1 |
Phonological Awareness | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Farver, Jo Ann M. | 1 |
Joshi, R. Malatesha | 1 |
McBride, Catherine | 1 |
Saiegh-Haddad, Elinor | 1 |
Wang, Ying | 1 |
Zhou, Yanling | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Grade 2 | 2 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Wang, Ying; McBride, Catherine; Zhou, Yanling; Joshi, R. Malatesha; Farver, Jo Ann M. – Journal of Research in Reading, 2018
How do native Chinese-speaking (CS) and non-Chinese-speaking (NCS) children learn to read and write in Chinese? In the present study, 29 CS and 34 NCS second and third graders aged 76 to 122 months (M = 93.65) participated in an experiment where they were taught 16 new Chinese characters in one of four conditions--copy, radical, phonological and…
Descriptors: Chinese, Native Speakers, Second Language Learning, Grade 2
Saiegh-Haddad, Elinor – Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2005
Arabic native speaking children are born into a unique linguistic context called diglossia (Ferguson, "word", 14, 47?56, [1959]). In this context, children grow up speaking a Spoken Arabic Vernacular (SAV), which is an exclusively spoken language, but later learn to read another linguistically related form, Modern Standard Arabic (MSA).…
Descriptors: Correlation, Reading Fluency, Semitic Languages, Phonological Awareness