Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Experimental Psychology | 2 |
Adults | 1 |
Age Differences | 1 |
Children | 1 |
Cognitive Processes | 1 |
Comparative Analysis | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Equipment | 1 |
Experiments | 1 |
Identification | 1 |
Innovation | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Beck, Sarah R. | 2 |
Apperly, Ian A. | 1 |
Chappell, Jackie | 1 |
Cutting, Nicola | 1 |
Guthrie, Carlie | 1 |
McColgan, Kerry L. T. | 1 |
Robinson, Elizabeth J. | 1 |
Rowley, Martin G. | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Reports - Research | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Elementary Education | 2 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Beck, Sarah R.; McColgan, Kerry L. T.; Robinson, Elizabeth J.; Rowley, Martin G. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Children's well-documented tendency to behave as if they know more than they do about uncertain events is reduced under two conditions: when the outcome of a chance event has yet to be determined and when one unknown outcome has occurred but is difficult to imagine. In Experiment 1, in line with published findings, 5- and 6-year-olds (N=61)…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Experimental Psychology, Student Behavior, Identification
Beck, Sarah R.; Apperly, Ian A.; Chappell, Jackie; Guthrie, Carlie; Cutting, Nicola – Cognition, 2011
Tool making evidences intelligent, flexible thinking. In Experiment 1, we confirmed that 4- to 7-year-olds chose a hook tool to retrieve a bucket from a tube. In Experiment 2, 3- to 5-year-olds consistently failed to innovate a simple hook tool. Eight-year-olds performed at mature levels. In contrast, making a tool following demonstration was easy…
Descriptors: Experiments, Children, Thinking Skills, Age Differences