NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Teachers10
Parents2
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Showing 211 to 225 of 231 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Graham, Susan A.; Kilbreath, Cari S.; Welder, Andrea N. – Child Development, 2004
This study examined the influence of shape similarity and labels on 13-month-olds' inductive inferences. In 3 experiments, 123 infants were presented with novel target objects with or without a nonvisible property, followed by test objects that varied in shape similarity. When objects were not labeled, infants generalized the nonvisible property…
Descriptors: Inferences, Infants, Nouns, Logical Thinking
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
French, Robert M.; Mareschal, Denis; Mermillod, Martial; Quinn, Paul C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2004
Disentangling bottom-up and top-down processing in adult category learning is notoriously difficult. Studying category learning in infancy provides a simple way of exploring category learning while minimizing the contribution of top-down information. Three- to 4-month-old infants presented with cat or dog images will form a perceptual category…
Descriptors: Infants, Classification, Cognitive Processes, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Trudeau, Natacha; Sutton, Ann; Dagenais, Emmanuelle; de Broeck, Sophie; Morford, Jill – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2007
Purpose: This study investigated the impact of syntactic complexity and task demands on construction of utterances using picture communication symbols by participants from 3 age groups with no communication disorders. Method: Participants were 30 children (7;0 [years;months] to 8;11), 30 teenagers (12;0 to 13;11), and 30 adults (18 years and…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Language Skills, Adolescents, Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Webb, Sara J.; Long, Jeffrey D.; Nelson, Charles A. – Developmental Science, 2005
The goal of the current study was to assess general maturational changes in the ERP in the same sample of infants from 4 to 12 months of age. All participants were tested in two experimental manipulations at each age: a test of facial recognition and one of object recognition. Two sets of analyses were undertaken. First, growth curve modeling with…
Descriptors: Infants, Recognition (Psychology), Memory, Longitudinal Studies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hollich, George; Newman, Rochelle S.; Jusczyk, Peter W. – Child Development, 2005
In 4 studies, 7.5-month-olds used synchronized visua-lauditory correlations to separate a target speech stream when a distractor passage was presented at equal loudness. Infants succeeded in a segmentation task (using the head-turn preference procedure with video familiarization) when a video of the talker's face was synchronized with the target…
Descriptors: Infants, Auditory Discrimination, Auditory Stimuli, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dannemiller, James L. – Developmental Science, 2005
Very young infants orient overtly with eye and head movements to salient events in their visual environments, but those events rarely occur in the absence of competing visual stimuli. Two different models of how this kind of orienting is related to number and distribution of elements in the stimulus field were tested with infants across the age…
Descriptors: Infants, Visual Stimuli, Nonverbal Communication, Eye Movements
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Richardson, Daniel C.; Kirkham, Natasha Z. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2004
The ability to keep track of locations in a dynamic, multimodal environment is crucial for successful interactions with other people and objects. The authors investigated the existence and flexibility of spatial indexing in adults and 6-month-old infants by adapting an eye-tracking paradigm from D. C. Richardson and M. J. Spivey (2000). Multimodal…
Descriptors: Infants, Eye Movements, Adults, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Richards, Lynn V.; Coventry, Kenny R.; Clibbens, John – Journal of Child Language, 2004
The effect of both geometric and extra-geometric factors on children's production of "in" is reported (free-response paradigm). Eighty children across four age groups (means 4;1, 5;5, 6;1, and 7;1) were shown video scenes of puppets placing real objects in various positions with reference to a bowl and a plate. Located objects were placed at three…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Young Children, Geometric Concepts, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Breitfelder, Leisa M. – TEACHING Exceptional Children Plus, 2008
Research-based information is used to support the idea of the use of adaptations and accommodations for early childhood students who have varying disabilities. Multiple adaptations and accommodations are outlined. A step-by-step plan is provided on how to make specific adaptations and accommodations to fit the specific needs of early childhood…
Descriptors: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, Learning Disabilities, Behavior Modification, Communication Disorders
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Blaga, Otilia M.; Colombo, John – Developmental Psychology, 2006
Young infants have repeatedly been shown to be slower than older infants to shift fixation from a midline stimulus to a peripheral stimulus. This is generally thought to reflect maturation of the neural substrates that mediate the disengagement of attention, but this developmental difference may also be attributable to young infants' slower…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Infants, Attention Control, Dimensional Preference
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Napolitano, Amanda C.; Sloutsky, Vladimir M. – Child Development, 2004
When presented simultaneously with equally discriminable, but unfamiliar, visual and auditory stimuli, 4-year-olds exhibited auditory dominance, processing only auditory information ( Sloutsky & Napolitano, 2003). The current study examined factors underlying auditory dominance. In 6 experiments, 4-year-olds (N181) were presented with auditory and…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Auditory Stimuli, Preschool Children, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Joh, Amy, S.; Adolph, Karen, E. – Child Development, 2006
Walkers fall frequently, especially during infancy. Children (15, 21, 27, 33, and 39 month-olds) and adults were tested in a novel foam pit paradigm to examine age-related changes in the relationship between falling and prospective control of locomotion. In trial 1, participants walked and fell into a deformable foam pit marked with distinct…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Experiential Learning, Accident Prevention, Motor Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Varol, Filiz; Colburn, Linda K. – AACE Journal, 2007
The purpose of this study was to investigate the critical attributes of mathematical software designed for children between the ages of four and seven. This study sought to offer guidelines that will assist software designers in the design of developmentally appropriate educational software. In addition, teachers and parents may benefit from this…
Descriptors: Computer Software, Young Children, Mathematics Instruction, Computer Uses in Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Loots, Gerrit; Devise, Isabel; Jacquet, Wolfgang – Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2005
This article presents a study that examined the impact of visual communication on the quality of the early interaction between deaf and hearing mothers and fathers and their deaf children aged between 18 and 24 months. Three communication mode groups of parent?deaf child dyads that differed by the use of signing and visual?tactile communication…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Parent Child Relationship, Deafness, Total Communication
Cohen-Maitre, Stacey Ann; Haerich, Paul – Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 2005
This study investigated the ability of color and motion to elicit and maintain visual attention in a sample of children with cortical visual impairment (CVI). It found that colorful and moving objects may be used to engage children with CVI, increase their motivation to use their residual vision, and promote visual learning.
Descriptors: Visual Learning, Attention, Visual Impairments, Children
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  6  |  7  |  8  |  9  |  10  |  11  |  12  |  13  |  14  |  15  |  16