Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 3 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 5 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 12 |
Descriptor
Sentences | 13 |
Verbs | 13 |
Language Acquisition | 5 |
Nouns | 5 |
Elementary School Students | 4 |
Grade 3 | 4 |
Young Children | 4 |
Evidence | 3 |
Form Classes (Languages) | 3 |
Grammar | 3 |
Kindergarten | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Alamargot, Denis | 1 |
Ambridge, Ben | 1 |
Artzi, Sigal | 1 |
Bartek, Brian | 1 |
Caporossi, Gilles | 1 |
Carter, Allyson | 1 |
Chang, Franklin | 1 |
Chilton, Molly Welsh | 1 |
Ehri, Linnea C. | 1 |
Fayol, Michel | 1 |
Fisher, Cynthia | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 12 |
Reports - Research | 11 |
Dissertations/Theses -… | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 13 |
Elementary Education | 8 |
Primary Education | 7 |
Grade 3 | 4 |
Kindergarten | 3 |
Grade 5 | 2 |
Intermediate Grades | 2 |
Middle Schools | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 12 | 1 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Location
China (Beijing) | 1 |
Greece | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Janna B. Oetting; Janet L. McDonald; Lori E. Vaughn – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2023
Purpose: Within General American English (GAE), the grammar weaknesses of children with developmental language disorder (DLD) have been documented with many tasks, including grammaticality judgments. Recently, Vaughn et al. replicated this finding with a judgment task targeting tense and agreement (T/A) structures for children who spoke African…
Descriptors: Grammar, Language Impairments, Developmental Delays, English
Zhang, Xiaowen; Zhou, Peng – First Language, 2022
It has been well-documented that although children around 4 years start to attribute false beliefs to others in classic false-belief tasks, they are still less able to evaluate the truth-value of propositional belief-reporting sentences, especially when belief conflicts with reality. This article investigates whether linguistic cues, verb…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Beliefs, Task Analysis, Sentences
Rothou, Kyriakoula M. – International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 2021
The study explored the inflectional morphological awareness of Greek-speaking poor comprehenders identified in grade 3. The sample consisted of 13 poor comprehenders and 27 good comprehenders. The poor comprehenders were selected from 126 children attended 19 primary schools in metropolitan area of Athens using a cut-off-based approach. All the…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Greek, Grade 3, Elementary School Students
Fitton, Lisa; Hoge, Rachel; Petscher, Yaacov; Wood, Carla – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2019
Purpose: The purpose of this study was (a) to examine the underlying components or factor structure of the Bilingual English-Spanish Assessment (BESA; Peña, Gutiérrez-Clellen, Iglesias, Goldstein, & Bedore, 2014) sentence repetition task and (b) to examine the relationship between Spanish-English speaking children's sentence repetition and…
Descriptors: Psychometrics, Bilingual Students, Bilingualism, English
Alamargot, Denis; Flouret, Lisa; Larocque, Denis; Caporossi, Gilles; Pontart, Virginie; Paduraru, Carmen; Morisset, Pauline; Fayol, Michel – Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2015
This study was designed to (1) investigate the procedure responsible for successful written subject-verb agreement, and (2) describe how it develops across grades. Students in Grades 3, 5 and 12 were asked to read noun-noun-verb sentences aloud (e.g., "Le chien des voisins mange" ["The dog of the neighbors eats"]) and write out…
Descriptors: Grade 3, Grade 5, Grade 12, Sentences
Karniol, Rachel; Artzi, Sigal; Ludmer, Maya – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2016
Third and 5th grade Hebrew-speaking children performed two sentence completion tasks, one requiring the assignment of male, female, or gender-ambiguous names and the inflection of verbs for male-stereotyped, female-stereotyped, and gender-neutral activities, and the other task, of inflecting verbs for male- and female-stereotyped activities…
Descriptors: Grammar, Gender Differences, Verbs, Semitic Languages
Rowland, Caroline F.; Chang, Franklin; Ambridge, Ben; Pine, Julian M.; Lieven, Elena V. M. – Cognition, 2012
Structural priming paradigms have been influential in shaping theories of adult sentence processing and theories of syntactic development. However, until recently there have been few attempts to provide an integrated account that explains both adult and developmental data. The aim of the present paper was to begin the process of integration by…
Descriptors: Priming, Evidence, Sentences, Verbs
Scott, Rose M.; Fisher, Cynthia – Cognition, 2012
Recent evidence shows that children can use cross-situational statistics to learn new object labels under referential ambiguity (e.g., Smith & Yu, 2008). Such evidence has been interpreted as support for proposals that statistical information about word-referent co-occurrence plays a powerful role in word learning. But object labels represent only…
Descriptors: Evidence, Sentences, Verbs, Figurative Language
Chilton, Molly Welsh; Ehri, Linnea C. – Reading Research Quarterly, 2015
An experiment compared the impact of more and less semantically connected sentence contexts on vocabulary learning. Third graders (N = 40) were taught the definitions and meanings of six unfamiliar verbs: "anticipate," "attain," "devise," "restrain," "wield," and "persist." The verbs were…
Descriptors: Vocabulary Development, Sentences, Semantics, Vignettes
Bartek, Brian; Lewis, Richard L.; Vasishth, Shravan; Smith, Mason R. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
Many comprehension theories assert that increasing the distance between elements participating in a linguistic relation (e.g., a verb and a noun phrase argument) increases the difficulty of establishing that relation during on-line comprehension. Such "locality effects" are expected to increase reading times and are thought to reveal properties…
Descriptors: Evidence, Sentences, Verbs, Eye Movements
Yuan, Sylvia Hsin Wei – ProQuest LLC, 2009
Children use syntax as well as observations of events to learn verb meanings. This is known as syntactic bootstrapping. This dissertation investigated the origins and mechanisms of syntactic bootstrapping. Prior evidence suggested that two-year-olds, but not younger children, could use aspects of sentence structure to assign different…
Descriptors: Verbs, Sentence Structure, Sentences, Semantics
Kazanina, Nina; Phillips, Colin – Cognition, 2007
Imperfective or progressive verb morphology makes it possible to use the name of a whole event to refer to an activity that is clearly not a complete instance of that event, leading to what is known as the Imperfective Paradox. For example, a sentence like "John was building a house" does not entail that a house ever got built. The Imperfective…
Descriptors: Verbs, Form Classes (Languages), Intervals, Sentences
Carter, Allyson; Gerken, Louann – Journal of Child Language, 2004
When English-speaking two-year-olds begin producing polysyllabic words, they often omit unstressed syllables that precede syllables with primary stress (Allen & Hawkins, 1980; Klein, 1981; Gerken, 1994a). One proposed mechanism for these omissions is that children omit syllables at a phonological level, due to prosodic constraints that act on…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Preschool Children, Sentences, Pronunciation