NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 10 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wiebke Klages; Magritt Lundestad; Paul Robert Sundar – International Journal of Mentoring and Coaching in Education, 2020
Purpose: The purpose of this paper is to present and discuss contrasting perceptions regarding "leadership and mentoring" among leaders of Norwegian early childhood education and care (ECEC) centres in their mentoring practices with newly qualified early childhood teachers (NQTs). Design/methodology/approach: Semi-structured qualitative…
Descriptors: Early Childhood Education, Child Care Centers, Early Childhood Teachers, Teacher Qualifications
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kimmel, Sue C. – Knowledge Quest, 2013
With all the discourse in school librarianship about collaboration, there is surprisingly little discussion of teacher planning. For teachers, planning is the taken-for-granted work necessary for teaching. Planning focuses on various increments of time, ranging from a single lesson to a day, a week, a grading term, and a school year. Teacher…
Descriptors: Lesson Plans, Cooperative Planning, Librarian Teacher Cooperation, Group Dynamics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
McGuigan, Nicola – Journal of Genetic Psychology, 2009
Previous studies have shown that young preschool children are highly sensitive to mutual engagement and struggle to diagnose the visibility of a figure when their facial area is occluded. The present study aimed to explore the specificity of engagement by varying (a) the orientation of a figure relative to an observer and (b) the visible area of…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Preschool Children, Attention, Orientation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lourenco, Stella F.; Addy, Dede; Huttenlocher, Janellen – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
It has been suggested that young children can only reorient, locating a target object, when the geometry of an enclosed space provides distinctive shape information [e.g., Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1994). A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children. "Nature," 370, 57-59]. Recently, however, young children were shown to specify…
Descriptors: Cues, Preschool Children, Measures (Individuals), Geometry
Copeland, Margaret Leitch; Brunette, Libby; Gimilaro, Susan – Exchange: The Early Childhood Leaders' Magazine Since 1978, 2008
Ten years ago when the first group of children aged out of the Applewood Learning Center's (Londonderry, New Hampshire) school-age child care program, parents asked the program to find a way to serve their young teenage children so that they would not be left home alone for the summer. The teens had friendships based on many years together in the…
Descriptors: Child Care, Summer Programs, Latchkey Children, Adolescents
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Repacholi, Betty M.; Meltzoff, Andrew N. – Child Development, 2007
Two experiments examined whether 18-month-olds learn from emotions directed to a third party. Infants watched an adult perform actions on objects, and an Emoter expressed Anger or Neutral affect toward the adult in response to her actions. The Emoter then became neutral and infants were given access to the objects. Infants' actions were influenced…
Descriptors: Infants, Toddlers, Psychological Patterns, Affective Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bahrick, Lorraine E.; Lickliter, Robert; Flom, Ross – Infancy, 2006
According to the intersensory redundancy hypothesis (IRH), during early development, perception of nonredundantly specified properties is facilitated in unimodal stimulation as compared with bimodal stimulation. Later in development, attention becomes more flexible and infants can detect nonredundantly specified properties in both unimodal and…
Descriptors: Hypothesis Testing, Stimulation, Infants, Redundancy
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bertin, Evelin; Bhatt, Ramesh S. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2006
Three-month-olds are sensitive to orientation changes of line drawings when they have a three-dimensional (3-D) interpretation and when the changes are defined by both 3-D depth and two-dimensional (2-D) picture plane cues [Bhatt, R. S., & Bertin, E. (2001). Pictorial cues and three-dimensional information processing in early infancy. Journal of…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Infants, Visual Discrimination, Visual Aids
Sullivan, Babs – Exchange: The Early Childhood Leaders' Magazine Since 1978, 2006
Using volunteers has a double benefit. The children get more individualized attention since there are more adults available to help in the classroom. Also, the volunteers see how much effort goes into making a classroom function and come away appreciating the efforts of center personnel. This article discusses the benefits of using community…
Descriptors: Volunteer Training, Volunteers, Parent Teacher Cooperation, Family School Relationship
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dannemiller, James L. – Developmental Science, 2005
Very young infants orient overtly with eye and head movements to salient events in their visual environments, but those events rarely occur in the absence of competing visual stimuli. Two different models of how this kind of orienting is related to number and distribution of elements in the stimulus field were tested with infants across the age…
Descriptors: Infants, Visual Stimuli, Nonverbal Communication, Eye Movements