Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 4 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 12 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 20 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 82 |
Descriptor
Child Development | 82 |
Familiarity | 82 |
Age Differences | 36 |
Infants | 26 |
Foreign Countries | 24 |
Preschool Children | 16 |
Young Children | 15 |
Children | 13 |
Developmental Stages | 13 |
Novelty (Stimulus Dimension) | 13 |
Visual Stimuli | 13 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 79 |
Reports - Research | 70 |
Reports - Evaluative | 5 |
Dissertations/Theses -… | 3 |
Information Analyses | 3 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 13 |
Preschool Education | 6 |
Elementary Education | 5 |
Higher Education | 4 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 2 |
Kindergarten | 2 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Secondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Australia | 5 |
United Kingdom | 4 |
Germany | 3 |
Finland | 2 |
California | 1 |
Cameroon | 1 |
Canada | 1 |
China | 1 |
Colorado (Denver) | 1 |
Florida | 1 |
Hong Kong | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
MacArthur Communicative… | 2 |
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 1 |
Program for International… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Neldner, Karri; Wilks, Matti; Crimston, Charlie R.; Jaymes, R. W. M.; Nielsen, Mark – Developmental Psychology, 2023
In industrialized societies, adults exhibit stable preferences for the types of people, animals, and entities they feel moral concern for (Crimston et al., 2016). Only one published study to date has utilized the moral circles paradigm to examine these preferences in children, finding that as children age, their preferences shift to become more…
Descriptors: Moral Values, Child Development, Familiarity, Preferences
Venus Ho; Emily Stonehouse; Ori Friedman – Developmental Psychology, 2024
Although stories for children often feature supernatural and fantastical events, children themselves often prefer realistic events when choosing what should happen in a story. In two experiments, we investigated whether 3- to 5-year-olds (total N = 240 from diverse backgrounds) might be more likely to include fantastical events in stories about…
Descriptors: Fiction, Fantasy, Child Development, Preferences
Nancekivell, Shaylene E.; Davidson, Natalie S.; Noles, Nicholaus S.; Gelman, Susan A. – Developmental Psychology, 2023
Defining developmental progressions can be an important step in identifying developmental precursors and mechanisms of change, within and across areas of reasoning. In one exploratory study, we examine whether the development of children's thinking about ownership follows a systematic progression wherein some components emerge reliably before…
Descriptors: Child Development, Developmental Stages, Ownership, Preschool Children
Jordan, Ashley E.; Wynn, Karen – Developmental Science, 2022
These studies investigate the influence of adults' explicit attention to commonalities of appearance on children's preference for individuals resembling themselves. Three findings emerged: (1) An adult's identification of two dolls' respective similarity to and difference from the child led 3-year-olds to prefer the similar doll (study 1, n = 32).…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Preferences, Familiarity, Social Cognition
Marc Colomer; Hyesung Grace Hwang; Nicole Burke; Amanda Woodward – Developmental Psychology, 2024
Presenting pictures of faces side by side is a common paradigm to assess infants' attentional biases according to social categories, such as gender, race, and language. However, seeing static faces does not represent infants' typical experience of the social world, which involves people in motion and performing actions. Here, we assessed infants'…
Descriptors: Infant Behavior, Behavior Patterns, Native Language, Second Language Learning
Williams, Allison J.; Danovitch, Judith H. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2022
As children get older, they become better able to discriminate between impossible and improbable statements and they realize that improbable events can occur in reality while impossible ones cannot. However, when children hear about extraordinary events from fictional entities (e.g., popular characters from children's media), they may be more…
Descriptors: Beliefs, Childrens Attitudes, Fantasy, Familiarity
Yasamin Motamedi; Margherita Murgiano; Beata Grzyb; Yan Gu; Viktor Kewenig; Ricarda Brieke; Ed Donnellan; Chloe Marshall; Elizabeth Wonnacott; Pamela Perniss; Gabriella Vigliocco – Child Development, 2024
Most language use is displaced, referring to past, future, or hypothetical events, posing the challenge of how children learn what words refer to when the referent is not physically available. One possibility is that iconic cues that imagistically evoke properties of absent referents support learning when referents are displaced. In an…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Child Development, Cues, Parent Child Relationship
Reschke, Peter J.; Fraser, Ashley M.; Picket, Janna; Workman, Katey; Lehnardt, Hans; Stockdale, Laura A.; Padilla-Walker, Laura M.; Cox, Kylin; Holmgren, Hailey G.; Hagen, Sophie; Summers, Kjersti; Clifford, Brandon N.; Essig, Liam W.; Coyne, Sarah M. – Developmental Psychology, 2023
Infants can help and share in the second year of life. However, there is limited knowledge as to variability in these behaviors as a function of target (e.g., caregiver vs. unfamiliar adult) and the influence of caregiver support on infant prosocial behavior. Infants (N = 268, 124 female) at 1-2 years of age (M = 1.47, SD = 0.27) and again at 2-3…
Descriptors: Infants, Toddlers, Helping Relationship, Sharing Behavior
Köksal, Özgün; Sodian, Beate – European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2023
Understanding that hypothesis testing is aimed at seeking information rather than producing desirable outcomes is indispensable for designing informative experiments. This study investigated the developmental course of information seeking compared to producing an effect in young children. In a between-subjects design, 4- to 6-year-olds (N = 109)…
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Information Seeking, Hypothesis Testing, Child Development
Von Holzen, Katie; Bergmann, Christina – Developmental Psychology, 2021
As they develop into mature speakers of their native language, infants must not only learn words but also the sounds that make up those words. To do so, they must strike a balance between accepting speaker-dependent variation (e.g., mood, voice, accent) but appropriately rejecting variation when it (potentially) changes a word's meaning (e.g., cat…
Descriptors: Infants, Pronunciation, Auditory Discrimination, Phonological Awareness
Hsin, Lisa B.; Gonzalez-Gomez, Nayeli; Barriere, Isabelle; Nazzi, Thierry; Legendre, Geraldine – Journal of Child Language, 2021
A surprising comprehension-production asymmetry in subject-verb (SV) agreement acquisition has been suggested in the literature, and recent research indicates that task-specific as well as language-specific features may contribute to this apparent asymmetry across languages. The present study investigates when during development children acquiring…
Descriptors: Spanish, Language Acquisition, Grammar, Language Variation
Kaisa Harju; Mari Vuorisalo; Maiju Paananen; Niina Rutanen – Early Years: An International Journal of Research and Development, 2024
This longitudinal multiple case study explores children's transitions within early childhood education and care (ECEC) in Finland. In ECEC children typically transition from one group or center to another. This study explores how physical, social and philosophical discontinuities and continuities constitute these transitions. Five focus children's…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Early Childhood Education, Early Childhood Teachers, Preschool Children
Graf Estes, Katharine; Antovich, Dylan M.; Hay, Jessica F. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2018
This research investigates the development of constraints in word learning. Previous experiments have shown that as infants gain more knowledge of native language structure, they become more selective about the forms that they accept as labels. However, the developmental pattern exhibited depends greatly on the way that infants are introduced to…
Descriptors: Barriers, Language Acquisition, Infants, Age Differences
Perszyk, Danielle R.; Ferguson, Brock; Waxman, Sandra R. – Developmental Science, 2018
The power of human language rests upon its intricate links to human cognition. By 3 months of age, listening to language supports infants' ability to form object categories, a building block of cognition. Moreover, infants display a systematic shift between 3 and 4 months--a shift from familiarity to novelty preferences--in their expression of…
Descriptors: Premature Infants, Language Acquisition, Cognitive Ability, Child Development
Yunus, Farhana Wan – Asian Journal of University Education, 2019
Research on children's peer interactions shows many benefits for children's development especially in developing children's social competence. Drawing on a case study data from a study that investigated peer interactions among under-three year old children in three Malaysian childcare centers, this paper provides a picture of how the children's…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Preschool Children, Peer Relationship, Interaction