Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 11 |
Descriptor
Child Development | 12 |
Young Children | 12 |
Age Differences | 6 |
Cognitive Development | 5 |
Intention | 4 |
Adults | 3 |
Child Behavior | 3 |
Interpersonal Communication | 3 |
Toddlers | 3 |
Child Language | 2 |
Cooperation | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Developmental Psychology | 3 |
Developmental Science | 3 |
Cognition | 2 |
Journal of Child Language | 2 |
Child Development | 1 |
Cognitive Development | 1 |
Author
Tomasello, Michael | 12 |
Carpenter, Malinda | 3 |
Rakoczy, Hannes | 3 |
Herrmann, Esther | 2 |
Behne, Tanya | 1 |
Call, Josep | 1 |
Dodson, Kelly | 1 |
Engelmann, Jan M. | 1 |
Grafenhain, Maria | 1 |
Grosse, Gerlind | 1 |
Hernandez-Lloreda, Victoria | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 12 |
Reports - Research | 8 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 3 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Germany | 2 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Rapp, Diotima J.; Engelmann, Jan M.; Herrmann, Esther; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Psychology, 2019
Reputational concerns are known to promote cooperation. Individuals regularly act more prosocially when their behavior is observable by others. Here, we investigate 4- and 5-year-old (N = 144) children's reputational strategies in a competitive group setting. The aim of the current study was to explore whether children's sharing behavior is…
Descriptors: Young Children, Reputation, Peer Groups, Sharing Behavior
Herrmann, Esther; Misch, Antonia; Hernandez-Lloreda, Victoria; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Science, 2015
Human beings have remarkable skills of self-control, but the evolutionary origins of these skills are unknown. Here we compare children at 3 and 6 years of age with one of humans' two nearest relatives, chimpanzees, on a battery of reactivity and self-control tasks. Three-year-old children and chimpanzees were very similar in their abilities to…
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Young Children, Animals, Animal Behavior
Schmerse, Daniel; Lieven, Elena; Tomasello, Michael – Journal of Child Language, 2015
We investigated whether children at the ages of two and three years understand that a speaker's use of the definite article specifies a referent that is in common ground between speaker and listener. An experimenter and a child engaged in joint actions in which the experimenter chose one of three similar objects of the same category to perform an…
Descriptors: Young Children, Child Language, Form Classes (Languages), Child Development
Grosse, Gerlind; Scott-Phillips, Thomas C.; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Psychology, 2013
Human cooperative communication involves both an informative intention that the recipient understands the content of the signal and also a (Gricean) communicative intention that the recipient recognizes that the speaker has an informative intention. The degree to which children understand this 2-layered nature of communication is the subject of…
Descriptors: Young Children, Interpersonal Communication, Intention, Cooperation
Schmidt, Marco F. H.; Rakoczy, Hannes; Tomasello, Michael – Cognition, 2012
To become cooperative members of their cultural groups, developing children must follow their group's social norms. But young children are not just blind norm followers, they are also active norm enforcers, for example, protesting and correcting when someone plays a conventional game the "wrong" way. In two studies, we asked whether young children…
Descriptors: Young Children, Norms, Child Development, Games
Melis, Alicia P.; Call, Josep; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Science, 2010
By three years of age, children are skilled at assessing under which circumstances others can see things. However, nothing is known about whether they can use this knowledge to guide their own deceptive behaviour. Here we investigated 3-year-olds' ability to strategically inhibit or conceal forbidden actions that a nearby adult experimenter could…
Descriptors: Cues, Visual Stimuli, Auditory Stimuli, Evaluation Methods
Vaish, Amrisha; Carpenter, Malinda; Tomasello, Michael – Child Development, 2010
Two studies investigated whether young children are selectively prosocial toward others, based on the others' moral behaviors. In Study 1 (N = 54), 3-year-olds watched 1 adult (the actor) harming or helping another adult. Children subsequently helped the harmful actor less often than a third (previously neutral) adult, but helped the helpful and…
Descriptors: Prosocial Behavior, Young Children, Moral Values, Intention
Rakoczy, Hannes; Tomasello, Michael – Cognition, 2009
Young children use and comprehend different kinds of speech acts from the beginning of their communicative development. But it is not clear how they understand the conventional and normative structure of such speech acts. In particular, imperative speech acts have a world-to-word direction of fit, such that their fulfillment means that the world…
Descriptors: Speech Acts, Young Children, Age Differences, Cognitive Development
Grafenhain, Maria; Behne, Tanya; Carpenter, Malinda; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Psychology, 2009
When adults make a joint commitment to act together, they feel an obligation to their partner. In 2 studies, the authors investigated whether young children also understand joint commitments to act together. In the first study, when an adult orchestrated with the child a joint commitment to play a game together and then broke off from their joint…
Descriptors: Young Children, Toddlers, Age Differences, Adults
Rakoczy, Hannes; Warneken, Felix; Tomasello, Michael – Cognitive Development, 2007
In theory of mind research, there is a long standing dispute about whether children come to understand the subjectivity of both desires and beliefs at the same time (around age 4), or whether there is an asymmetry such that desires are understood earlier. To address this issue, 3-year olds' understanding of situations in which two persons have…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Young Children, Child Development, Beliefs
Tomasello, Michael; Carpenter, Malinda – Developmental Science, 2007
We argue for the importance of processes of shared intentionality in children's early cognitive development. We look briefly at four important social-cognitive skills and how they are transformed by shared intentionality. In each case, we look first at a kind of individualistic version of the skill--as exemplified most clearly in the behavior of…
Descriptors: Socialization, Cognitive Development, Intention, Child Development

Dodson, Kelly; Tomasello, Michael – Journal of Child Language, 1998
Examined the role of animacy and pronouns as children ages 2 to 3 years acquired transitive construction. Participants learned two nonce verbs, one of which was modeled in several transitive sentence frames and the other in neutral sentence frames. Many children produced transitive sentences with the first verb, but only children near age 3…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Development, Child Language, English