Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Nouns | 9 |
Eye Movements | 5 |
Sentences | 5 |
Verbs | 5 |
Experiments | 4 |
Language Processing | 4 |
Reading Processes | 4 |
Syntax | 3 |
Error Patterns | 2 |
Grammar | 2 |
Memory | 2 |
More ▼ |
Author
Staub, Adrian | 9 |
Rayner, Keith | 3 |
Pollatsek, Alexander | 2 |
Clifton, Charles | 1 |
Clifton, Charles, Jr. | 1 |
Drieghe, Denis | 1 |
Grant, Margaret | 1 |
Hyona, Jukka | 1 |
Majewski, Helen | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Staub, Adrian; Grant, Margaret; Clifton, Charles, Jr.; Rayner, Keith – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
In this brief rejoinder, we respond to Farmer, Monaghan, Misyak, and Christiansen (2011). We argue that the data still do not support the claim that reading time is affected by the phonological typicality of a word for its part of speech. We also question Farmer et al.'s claim that interleaving syntactic structures in an experiment modifies…
Descriptors: Agricultural Occupations, Syntax, Reading, Phonology
Staub, Adrian – Cognition, 2010
Speakers are known to make subject-verb agreement errors both when a number-mismatching noun intervenes between the head of the subject phrase and the verb (e.g., "*The key to the cabinets are on the table") and in configurations in which there is a number-mismatching noun that does not intervene (e.g., "*The cabinets that the key open are on the…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Verbs, Nouns, Grammar
Staub, Adrian – Cognition, 2010
It is well known that sentences containing object-extracted relative clauses (e.g., "The reporter that the senator attacked admitted the error") are more difficult to comprehend than sentences containing subject-extracted relative clauses (e.g., "The reporter that attacked the senator admitted the error"). Two major accounts of this phenomenon…
Descriptors: Language Patterns, Sentences, Verbs, Eye Movements
Staub, Adrian – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
Speakers frequently make subject-verb number agreement errors in the presence of a local noun with a different number from the head of the subject phrase. A series of four experiments used a two-choice response time (RT) paradigm to investigate how the latency of correct agreement decisions is modulated by the presence of a number attractor, and…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Nouns, Syntax, Error Patterns
Drieghe, Denis; Pollatsek, Alexander; Staub, Adrian; Rayner, Keith – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2008
The distribution of landing positions and durations of first fixations in a region containing a noun preceded by either an article (e.g., the soldiers) or a high-frequency 3-letter word (e.g., all soldiers) were compared. Although there were fewer first fixations on the blank space between the high-frequency 3-letter word and the noun than on the…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Nouns, Human Body, Reading Processes
Staub, Adrian; Rayner, Keith; Pollatsek, Alexander; Hyona, Jukka; Majewski, Helen – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2007
Readers' eye movements were monitored as they read sentences containing noun-noun compounds that varied in frequency (e.g., elevator mechanic, mountain lion). The left constituent of the compound was either plausible or implausible as a head noun at the point at which it appeared, whereas the compound as a whole was always plausible. When the head…
Descriptors: Sentences, Eye Movements, Nouns, Experiments
Staub, Adrian – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Two eye movement experiments examined effects on syntactic reanalysis when the correct analysis was briefly entertained at an earlier point in the sentence. In Experiment 1, participants read sentences containing a noun phrase coordination/clausal coordination ambiguity, while in Experiment 2 they read sentences containing a subordinate clause…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Sentences, Nouns, Eye Movements
Staub, Adrian – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2007
Several previous studies (B. C. Adams, C. Clifton, & D. C. Mitchell, 1998; D. C. Mitchell, 1987; R. P. G. van Gompel & M. J. Pickering, 2001) have explored the question of whether the parser initially analyzes a noun phrase that follows an intransitive verb as the verb's direct object. Three eye-tracking experiments examined this issue in more…
Descriptors: Verbs, Nouns, Language Processing, Phrase Structure
Staub, Adrian; Clifton, Charles – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2006
Readers' eye movements were monitored as they read sentences in which two noun phrases or two independent clauses were connected by the word or (NP-coordination and S-coordination, respectively). The word either could be present or absent earlier in the sentence. When either was present, the material immediately following or was read more quickly,…
Descriptors: Nouns, Eye Movements, Sentence Structure, Reading Processes